NEUROBIOLOGY OF THE CHILDHOOD
ADVERSITY
MODEL AND SUBCLINICAL
EMOTIONAL
PARENTAL NEGLECT
NEUROBIOLOGÍA
DEL MODELO DE ADVERSIDAD
INFANTIL
Y NEGLIGENCIA PARENTAL EMOCIONAL
SUBCLÍNICA

Fabiano
de Abreu Agrela Rodrigues
Centro
de Pesquisa e Análises Heráclito (CPAH) Departamento de Física
Adriel
Pereira da Silva
Universidad
Europea del Atlántico
pág. 7298
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.37811/cl_rcm.v9i4.19331
Neurobiology
of the childhood adversity model and subclinical emotional
parental
neglect
Fabiano
de Abreu Agrela Rodrigues 1
adrielpsilva@gmail.com

https://orcid.org/0009
-0003-1157-8318
Centro
de Pesquisa e Análises Heráclito (CPAH)
Departamento
de Física
Adriel
Pereira da Silva
contato@cpah.com.br

https://orcid.org/0009
-0003-1157-8318
Universidad
Europea del Atlántico
ABSTRACT

Introduct:
Research in neurogenetics and human neurobiology, through the use of functional
neuroimaging
with proton emission (fMRI), we describe in an objective and analytical way, a clinical
set
based on neurobiology and social neurosciences, which organizes a series of situations by the
individuation
of objectivity and subjectivity, such as the current Syndrome Z. The most predominant
neurological
mechanisms are biobehaviors, all dependent on the neurological component, and develop
in
response to the quality of affective attention of biological parents, and present a hierarchical nosology
of
cumulative causal factors that were delimited from descriptive clinical practice, like all most diseases
in
Medicine. Methods: We present a clinical pre-diagnosis construct, based on neuropathophysiology,
which
was carried out in the practical field, and helps in the understanding and organized identification
of
a serious public health problem, and a hidden setback in family courts. It aims to contribute to the
problem
of NP, and its medical and specialized characterization, as well as its already known social and
psychological
consequences. Results: The quality of care is directly reflected in maladaptive
neurological
development, with deficits in emotional regulation, in intelligence and family and social
skills,
family synchrony and chronic states of cognition and behavior that distort the real values of
mental
health and the importance of interpersonal relationships (IR), Discussion: Several pieces of
evidence
have shown that complex PTSD or a history of childhood abuse (or both), and also in the
presence
of four or more situations of Adverse Emotions in Childhood (ACE), negatively impact
parenting
and multiple comorbidities, physical and mental, and social risk behaviors, in addition to
generating
"intergenerational cycles" of trauma. Conclusion: This study should be reflected upon by
professionals
in family law and Family Medicine, as there is no assessment and much less protective
care
interventions for child neurodevelopment , which is sensitive to ACEs, which determine or limit
the
social, mental and organic life of the child.
Keywords
: z syndrome, metabolic syndrome, hypodopaminergic, addiction, dopamine
1
Autor Principal
Correspondencia:
adrielpsilva@gmail.com
pág. 7299
Neurobiología
del modelo de adversidad infantil y negligencia parental
emocional
subclínica
RESUMEN

Introducción: La investigación en neurogenética y neurobiología humana, a través del uso de
neuroimagen funcional con emisión de protones (fMRI), nos permite describir de manera objetiva y
analítica un conjunto clínico basado en la neurobiología y las neurociencias sociales, que organiza una
serie de situaciones mediante la individuación de la objetividad y la subjetividad, como es el caso del
actual Síndrome Z. Los mecanismos neurológicos más predominantes son los biocomportamientos,
todos dependientes del componente neurológico, que se desarrollan en respuesta a la calidad de la
atención afectiva de los padres biológicos, y que presentan una nosología jerárquica de factores causales
acumulativos delimitados a partir de la práctica clínica descriptiva, como ocurre con la mayoría de las
enfermedades en Medicina. Métodos: Se presenta un constructo de prediagnóstico clínico, basado en la
neuropatofisiología, que fue elaborado en el campo práctico y que contribuye a la comprensión e
identificación organizada de un grave problema de salud pública, además de ser un obstáculo oculto en
los tribunales de familia. Este modelo busca aportar a la problemática del NP y a su caracterización
médica y especializada, así como a sus ya conocidas consecuencias sociales y psicológicas. Resultados:
La calidad del cuidado se refleja directamente en un desarrollo neurológico desadaptativo, con déficits
en la regulación emocional, en la inteligencia y en las habilidades familiares y sociales, en la sincronía
familiar y en estados crónicos de cognición y comportamiento que distorsionan los valores reales de la
salud mental y la importancia de las relaciones interpersonales (RI). Discusión: Diversas evidencias han
mostrado que el TEPT complejo o los antecedentes de abuso infantil (o ambos), así como la presencia
de cuatro o más situaciones de Experiencias Adversas en la Infancia (ACE), impactan negativamente
en la crianza y en múltiples comorbilidades físicas, mentales y conductas de riesgo social, además de
generar “ciclos intergeneracionales” de trauma. Conclusión: Este estudio debe ser considerado por los
profesionales del derecho de familia y de la Medicina Familiar, dado que actualmente no existen
evaluaciones ni mucho menos intervenciones de cuidado protector para el neurodesarrollo infantil, el
cual es sensible a las ACE y determina o limita la vida social, mental y orgánica del niño.

Palabras clave: síndrome Z, síndrome metabólico, hipodopaminérgico, adicción, dopamina

Artículo
recibido 20 julio 2025
Aceptado
para publicación: 20 agosto 2025
pág. 7300
INTRODUCTION

The
care of children and adolescents is culturally and legally the responsibility of the parents who have
custody
and the coexistence of these children is the potentially most protective and appropriate space
for
their development. The community and the State carry out complementary protection, information,
and
collective policies, as established by Brazilian legislation, always aiming at well-being and thus
ensuring
the best possible state of health. 1 The expression “potentially more protective” reflects that
although
there is an expectation regarding the care offered within families, it is also known that this is
not
always an effective space of protection, and the family environment can also be hostile, with
violati
ons of children's rights that are often hidden and vices. 1 The violations are diverse and multiple:
physical,
psychological, sexual violence, abandonment, and neglect. Parental neglect (PN) is the most
reported
type of violence and the one that most motivates the removal of children and adolescents from
their
family context 1 . According to the World Health Organization (WHO -2020), PN corresponds to
the
failure of parents or caregivers to ensure the child's development, whether in terms of health,
education,
shelter, nutrition and safe living conditions. Child abuse can also be defined as the failure to
provide
care when protective resources are available and which have repercussions on one or more areas
of
development. 2 According to the WHO, child abuse is defined as: "Child abuse or mistreatment
includes
all forms of physical and/or emotional mistreatment, sexual abuse, neglect or negligent
treatment,
or commercial exploitation, resulting in actual or potential harm to the health, survival,
development
or dignity of the child, in the context of a relationship of responsibility, trust or power ." 2
In
the scientific reality, particularly behavioral neurobiology studies, there is a voluminous and
overwhelming
amount of evidence on childhood adversity and presents an accumulation of clinical data
on
damage in various dimensions of children's mental health (MH), such as psychological, social,
neurological,
genetic, epigenetic, hormonal, inflammatory and psychiatric. 2-7 However, there are no
objective
and precise studies, combined with the hidden flaw of methodological preference for research
dependent
on statistics. There are no initiatives and concern from responsible communities in organizing
minimum
care, even if reductionist, and the reality of the epidemic of family conflicts and Mental
Disorders
(MD) that are influenced by psychosocial factors, shows a progressive and subtle increase,
pág. 7301
especially
after the Covid-19 epidemic. 8-12 Currently, in the basic health network in Brazil, there is
no
attention to mental health, in addition to persistent professional narratives, which feed the addictive
culture
of underestimating MD, often justified by lack of significant evidence, or belief in complex
problems
that are difficult to solve, and thus there is a lack of attention to mental health, which is as
important
as organic health, but with different values. 8-12 The lack of technical, in-depth and specialized
knowledge
in the area of family and worker health is evidence of misinformation, disorganization and
outdated
scientific knowledge, which is not a priority in public health policies, in addition to
professio
nal lack of motivation to conduct research and actions, and social conflicts are directly related
to
social disorders such as family crimes, domestic violence, and parental alienation, which are reflected
in
workers' health and public spending. 13-17 Currently, Medicine does not professionally address family
MS
in a technical and comprehensive manner, and currently Family Law is causing lasting
complications
in children's health, in several cases of family conflicts that are neurodysfunctional and
even
pathological, due to the lack of a medical approach, combined with the lack of understanding and
sense
of the real problem or concrete basis, which only the doctor has the ability to identify the diseases.
13
-17 The central focus is the health and rights of children and families, who are suffering from negligence
and
professional imprudence on the part of lawyers, and negligence and incompetence on the part of
doctors,
as there are no practical plans for care of MS in the family. 13-17 Neurodysfunctional or
neuromaladaptive
behaviors present emotional and affective dysfunctions, which have neurological
predominance
in their mechanisms, together with hormonal dysfunctions and long-lasting epigenetic
mechanisms,
since the child presents active neurodevelopment until the age of eight, and adverse
emotions
should be avoided. 18-25 However, with advances in recent years in research on neurogenetics
and
human neurobiology, through the use of functional neuroimaging with proton emission (fMRI), we
describe
objectively and analytically a clinical set based on neurobiology and social neurosciences,
which
organizes a series of situations by the individuation of objectivity and subjectivity, such as the
current
Syndrome Z. The most predominant neurological mechanisms and those with the greatest
clinical
presentation are biobehaviors, all dependent on the neurological component, and develop in
response
to the quality of affective attention from biological parents, and present a hierarchical nosology
pág. 7302
of
cumulative causal factors that were delimited from descriptive clinical practice, like most diseases in
Medicine.
26-32
Neurodysfunctional
Interpersonal Relationships in the Family Environment/Syndrome Z
According
to Leisman G et al, children are suffering harm as a natural consequence of universal child
development
and therefore NOT a consequence of variations in child-rearing practices and financial
status,
but rather initially resulting from the processing of components of affective attention, with
economic
and educational factors being built in a posteriori, and consequently resulting in specifically
predictable
behavioral patterns. 33-45 The activity of the human mind has been increasingly
demonstrated
to depend on the functioning of the neurobiological complex that consists of natural
phenomena
involving neuronal activities, hormones, action potentials, neurotransmitters, genetic
products
and others. 33-45
Human
consciousness presents objective (neurobiological) and subjective components in which each
person
has a unique narrative of how volition, habituation, performance capacity and the environment
interact
and influence over time, and thus provide experiences and learning. 33-45 A better understanding
of
the presented network may help advance treatments for neuropsychiatric conditions related to
aberrant
predictive processing and promote cognitive improvement in healthy individuals. 47-51 The
neurocognitive
variable related to the development of executive attention is called effortful control,
representing
the ability of the infant or an adult to inhibit a dominant response to execute a subdominant
response.
47-51 Being responsible for an action is to have the sense, notion and judgment, in the sense
of
feeling the effect of a means or stimulus ( whether positive or negative) and the reactions of the causal
chains
that led to the existence of the conscious intention that determined the action, whether active or
passive,
as well as self-control. 47-51 Individual values are personal convictions with a sense of
consideration
and importance, which generate significant motivation to seek or do, and can be derived
from
culture, belief, beauty, ethics, morals and others . 47-51 Interests are related to the pleasure of
doing
something and are reflected in a preference for certain activities over others, and may or may not
be
congruent with values . 47-51 Volition Is the cognitive and behavioral process by which an individual
decides
to perform an action that he or she desires. It is defined as a psychological function of mental
state,
and a neurological function of deliberate effort . 47-51 A complex, continuous and standardized
pág. 7303
volitional
process that involves the causality of personal cognitive acts and states, values and interests,
which
can be neurodysfunctional and even pathological. 52-56 When determining a choice to do an
activity,
this state is called imminent volition , and when we execute it, this act is called immanent ,
executive
or imperative volition. 52-56 When an immanent or determined choice controls or governs a
series
of voluntary acts, we classify it as a state of predominant volition, while subordinate volitions are
those
acts of will that put into practice the object desired by the predominant or governing volition. 57-
67
Volitional processes can be applied consciously and can be automated as habits over time, therefore it
consists
of a state and an act, such as a thought or a behavior. 57-67 Several neuroscientists and
researchers
of the psychic apparatus have highlighted the importance of the defense mechanisms of the
human
mind, but a comorbidity has emerged that is exclusive to these, which affects interpersonal
relationships
and has an elementary function, Emotional Survival. 68-69 The presence of family
synchrony
is fundamental and necessary for the adequate neurodevelopment of the entire emotional
cascade
of the newborn. 68-69 When affective attention is absent or deficient, a series of maladaptive
mechanisms
occur in the child's brain in response, which have the primary function of avoiding the
initial
discomfort of the absence of family synchrony, with amygdala behaviors, and a series of
associated
dysfunctions described below occur. 68-69 A set of eight Common Neurodysfunctions
(ONDs
) has been identified and described in clinical practice, which in a hierarchical construction,
represent
a initial microstructure of the personality, as it precedes and contributes to the onset of the
development
of some psychopathologies and some Personality Disorders (PD). 68-69 Thus, an ONCs
microstructure
is initially formed, is clearly delimited, with several neuromaladaptive
pathophysiological
mechanisms, and expresses neurocognitive and behavioral states with affective
attention
deficit, and simultaneously involuntary affective neglect that is hidden in several cases. The
microstructure
can evolve with internalizing or externalizing states and typical association with different
types
of stress. 68-69 Several empirical evidences have established that the long-term response profiles
of
the stress system are programmed to correspond to the quality of the social and physical environments
experienced
during the first sensitive windows of life. The following are the clinical and neurobiological
elements
of ONCs:
pág. 7304
(1)
Neurological Familial Synchrony Deficit (DNSF),
(2)
and limbic neuromaladjustment ( Family Schemas)
(3)
Hormonal and immunoinflammatory dysfunctions,
(4)
Dysfunctions of family neuropsychodynamics and neuromirroring,
(5)
Neurodysfunction of the reward system and dopamine homeostasis,
(6)
Neurodysfunctions of neural networks with emotional Engrams, with deficits in primitive
and
higher mental functions ( Neuroschematic Survival Syndrome)
(7)
Simultanagnosia 1 and cognitive limitation,
(8)
Dissociative Disorders of Neuroadaptive Consciousness
They
are subtle and automatic states such as Alexithymia 2 , Anosognosia 3 , Anosodiaphoria 4 ,
dissociation
of Consciousness, deficit of cognitive intelligence due to secondary simultanagnosia,
deviant
behaviors, secondary to pathological activated neurobehaviors , such as family schemes,
automatisms,
fear and stress. They produce illusions of emotional survival.
1
) Secondary Simultanagnosia: Inability to effectively identify more than four objects at the same
time.

2
) Secondary Alexithymia : Inability to effectively self-observe (self-identify) emotional and affective
awareness
at the same time.
3
) Secondary Anosognosia: Inability to effectively identify another person's emotional and affective
state.

4
) Anosodiaphoria : Inability to identify one's own health condition, associated with denial of one's
state.
The neural mechanisms underlying these difficulties are the same in individuals with
dysfunctional
parenting, and other traits of PD and family and occupational social RINs. In the
current
literature, there is a growing number of clinical methods of emotional measurement, which
analyze
using physiological markers, associated with other underlying markers that influence the
pathophysiology
of physical and mental clinical diseases, and in addition to social interfaces and
disorders.
68-69 According to Miglin R et al., there is a growing number of transdiagnostic clinical
approaches
that evaluate behavioral functioning in real time, and thus it is possible to measure the
emotional
socialization of parents, together with children's emotionality, as well as the quality and
pág. 7305
effectiveness
of these interactions in everyday life. 70-73 A history of childhood adversity should be
considered
in the differential diagnosis of developmental delay, dyslexia, dermatitis, asthma, recurrent
infections,
somatization, sleep disorders, but also for psychological and behavioral functioning similar
to
Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) and ADHD. 70-73 With a prolonged ontogeny, relatively high
levels
of glucocorticoid receptors, and persistent postnatal neurogenesis, the hippocampus is particularly
vulnerable
to the effects of stress. 70-73
Engrams

Neural
representations of context, dependent on repeated activation of neural ensembles, remain stable
over
a period of time to facilitate recall and integration. 7477 An engram is a significant and lasting
physical
or chemical change in a neural network due to activity in a subset of neurons caused by episodic
stimuli,
which can be reactivated upon presentation of all or part of the original stimulus ensemble,
leading
to memory retrieval. 7477 Furthermore, engrams can be described as networks of individual
engram
cells that act together to store the components of memories, and reactivation of this network is
necessary
and sufficient for recall of that encoded memory. 7477 An engram can be understood as the
physical
record of a representation, with the activated ensemble becoming reactivatable within a given
stimulus
ensemble (context), resulting in recall. 7477 Engram activation may even be sufficient to induce
a
false memory in the form of generalization of a fear response in one context to a neutral context or
even
to construct a representation of a context that has never been physically experienced.7477 Kitamura
et
al. found that engrams are actually formed simultaneously in the early stage of context memory
acquisition
in both the HPC and the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), where long-term memory is
stored.
7477 The Standard Consolidation Model states that newly acquired episodic memories are
initially
stored only in the HPC and then transferred to the cortex as long-term memories, accompanied
by
the “deletion” of engrams from the H PC over time. 7477 The The mPFC engram cells that were
generated
in the early stage are not reactivated during the recent memory retrieval state but exist as
“silent
engrams.” 7477 These silent engrams become functionally mature over time, concurrent with
the
transition of memory to the basolateral amygdala (BLA), which is required for fear memory, while
HPC
engram cells gradually become silent over time. 7477 Matos et al. demonstrated that the activity
of
a small subset of mPFC neurons is sufficient and necessary for the expression of remote memory,
pág. 7306
and
selective disruption of cAMP response element binding protein (CREB) function in mPFC engram
cells
after CFC impairs remote memory formation. Several studies have demonstrated that neurons
activated
by experience play a key role in memory. Learning-activated neurons are a cellular substrate
for
memory storage and retrieval and constitute engram cells. 8088 The relatively low overlap between
sets
of neurons activated during learning and recall (1040%) shows how engrams change throughout
memory
consolidation, through the dynamism of neurons ‘leaving’ or ‘entering’ the engram. 8088
Knowledge
of the temporal profile of engrams elucidates how their composition is related to mnemonic
properties,
such as memory selectivity, an essential characteristic for adaptive behavior. Tomé DF et
al
., demonstrated through a spiking neuronal network model, that memory engrams are dynamic and
that
changes in their composition are mediated by inhibitory plasticity. 8088 Thus , they are fundamental
for
the emergence of memory selectivity, during recall and memory consolidation. 8088 Fear
generalization
is a maladaptive response to harmless stimuli or situations that is characteristic of post-
traumatic
stress disorder and other anxiety disorders. 80-88 Competition between engram and non-
engram
cells in the dentate gyrus determines fear generalization. The overgeneralized fear response to
innocuous
cues may result from oversensitized memory retrieval resulting from dysfunctional activity
in
the hippocampus. 80-88 Currently , research on engrams is growing. Eligible neurons within a given
brain
region have been shown to compete for allocation to an engram, and relative neuronal excitability
determines
the outcome of this competition, which determines how the engrams interact. Studies have
shown
long-lasting changes in engram cells such as increased synaptic strength and spine density in
resting
-state (offline) neurons, as well as preferential connectivity with other downstream engram cells .
89
-90 Therefore, both increased intrinsic excitability and synaptic plasticity act simultaneously in
engram
formation, in addition to participating in memory consolidation and retrieval processes. . 89-90
Technological
advances have allowed targeting of individual cell subtypes through gene expression,
which
can include constitutively active proteins or more transient expression caused by neuronal
activity
.89-90 According to Albarran et al., increasing the stability of neocortical dendritic spines
improves
learning, while decreasing the size of neocortical dendritic spines leads to impaired
performance
in a motor task . . 89-90 Recently, it has become possible to label synapses between neurons ,
thanks
to GFP Reconstitution Across Synaptic Partners (GRASP), which was adapted for pre- and
pág. 7307
postsynaptic
engram neurons with a genetic method based on the Immediate Early Gene (IEG) cFos
and
allowed visualizing the engram at the synaptic level . 89-90 However, the genetic system based on
the
cFos IEG provides only transient labeling and it is unclear whether other genetic schemes based on
other
IEGs would aid in the labeling of structural synaptic engrams. . 89-90 Murthy BKB et al .,
demonstrated
through the use of the GRASP system in the dorsal hippocampus of mice, under the
control
of two different IEG promoters, commonly used to identify neuronal engrams: cFos and Arc ,
and
demonstrated the possibility of identifying and labeling functional engrams for different periods. .
89
-90
States
chronic neuro determinists
Second
Vygotsky, the human mind has two types of functions: elementary psychological and higher
psychological.
The human species is essentially social, as it is only through interpersonal interactions
that
the development of humanization occurs. 91-93 The microstructure of the ONCs emotional survival
personality
presents a series of dysfunctions of the higher and primitive psychic functions, which
function
as neural networks, which are true engrams of maladaptive emotional systems, which are
synonymous
with neuronal signatures or cognitive and behavioral schemes, and present dysfunction of
neuronal
hyperconnectivity.91-93 Such cognitive states with associated dysfunctional alterations are
subtle
and subtle, and can generate micro-illusions, dissociation of consciousness, self-deceptions,
choices,
convenient values of emotional survival, which impair neurosignification 91-93 . The set is
composed
of chronic neuroschematic activations, with variations of dysfunctions of general intelligence,
executive
function, processing speed, volition, emotional memory, reasoning, perceptive, verbal
comprehension,
interpersonal communication deficit, language dysfunction, fixation of survival values,
family
and occupational RINs. 91-93 In a second plane of cognitive dysfunctions, which are objective
and
involuntary in onset, permeate the sense of voluntarism in what is presented in our everyday
consciousness,
in which a group of neuro-schemas of memories, of psychosocial coping ( social coping).
94
-95 The routine functioning of these underlying network systems and subnetworks, can present
functional
and neurofunctional networks working simultaneously, in which represents emotional
Neurodeterminism.
94-95
pág. 7308
Neuro
biopsychosocial conditioning
Biopsychosocial
conditioning in the family environment, it represents the activity of several schemes
simultaneously,
for long periods, as they are activated by the family context, with environmental or
family
clues. 94-95 They present function of coping with biopsychosocial stress, and begin with sudden
onsets
of amygdala hyperactivity, which produces a narrowing of consciousness, since the amygdala
system
is the main component of momentary Mastery and Control and its maintenance, as well as being
responsible
for sudden emotional defenses and excess reason that brings resistance and rigidity, making
irrational
interaction or behavior, which is illusory and based on belief, non-self-perceptible. 94-95 The
amygdala
system expresses the clinical schemas in the family environment, habituated in a
dysfunctional
way in self-attention, presenting insufficiency of interoception ( alexithymia), cognitive
inflexibility
of correlating several pieces of knowledge simultaneously (secondary simultanagnosia). 96-
98
Presence of neuronal hyperconnectivity dysfunction in value -oriented attentional capture (VDAC),
or
fixation, associated with attention deficit in interpersonal and written communication. 9698 A large
body
of literature suggests that neural activity consists of predictions and prediction errors that are
common
throughout the brain, and thus the vast majority of fMRI experiments in affective neuroscience
are
designed to limit predictability, including clinical data from visual, auditory, somatosensory, and
motivated
behaviors.9698 Predictions are important for emotional and material survival control
function,
but they can be derived from semantic cues (verbal instruction) or recently learned associations
stored
in memory (conditioning) . 9698 The ascending connections of the Superior Colliculus are
candidates
for an early plastic effect on higher, more “experience-dependent” circuits of the social brain.
96
98 Evidence shows that the Superior Colliculus contains the largest amounts of GABA and the highest
density
of GABAergic synaptic terminals in the brain . 9698 They can refer to stimulus attributes (from
low
-level sensory features to object perception to complex social interactions), stimulus timing (even
when
“jittery”), expected motor responses (button presses, eye movements), expected somatosensory
stimulation
(sensation of pressing a button), predicted stimulus sampling given motor responses (eye
movements,
pupil dilation), and also visceral changes (changes in heart rate, respiration), and among
others
9698 Given that the prestimulus period likely consists of different brain states at different times
(rather
than a stable baseline), this raises the question of whether the prestimulus brain state has
pág. 7309
implications
for the brainbehavior relationship. 9698 The brain is in constant motion, and when a
stimulus
disrupts its trajectory, the functional activity observed in response to the stimulus reflects a
deviation
from the trajectory. 9698 The predictions developed over the years vary among individuals
as
a function of their personal developmental history and are likely to be difficult to modify. 9698
Research
in animals has shown that early sensory areas exhibit plasticity related to valence processing,
which
is considered a “core” affective feature of our emotional lives. 9698 Alternatively, a predictive
processing
account suggests that greater activity in these early areas may be driven by prediction error
at
relatively lower levels of the hierarchy. 9698 In typical picture-viewing task designs in affective
neuroscience,
low-level predictions might concern when and where to position the eyes, how much to
dilate
the pupils, and the expected changes in visual sensory information given these motor movements
(
“active inferences”), as generated from eye movements . 99101 To the extent that affect involves a
multilevel
cascade of predictions and prediction errors, it is quite possible that predictions are generated
in
sensory cortical areas other than those that receive direct input. 99101 An evocative image of a
screaming
child might drive predictions in the auditory cortex, and these predictions might play a
constitutive
role in affective experience. 99101 A psychological state might engage different brain regions
at
different times (degeneration) depending on how a stimulus disrupts (introduces prediction error into)
ongoing
brain activity. 99-101 The complete set of predictions constitutes a person's internal model and
may
be idiosyncratic to a person's experience. Direct inference involves inferring the probability of
activation
in a brain region given the previous brain state , in addition to the psychological state induced
by
a stimulus. 99-101 Chikazoe et al ., employed representational similarity analysis and identified brain
regions
that exhibit continuous activity and reflect a continuous dimension of subjective valence,
spanning
neurological states from negative to positive affect. While simple feature detection models
show
that there is a specific and reliable pattern of activation, and therefore underlies affective
experience,
located in limbic and paralimbic regions. 99-101 Because predictions are not stationary and
because
prediction errors are naturally contingent on predictions, the neural activity underlying affective
experience
will depend on which aspects of an evocative stimulus are meaningful, and present ongoing
pred
ictions of threat or violation. 99101 Current models of predictive processing suggest that there
may
be many different brain states that give rise to feelings of (dis )pleasure or arousal. The idea that
pág. 7310
the
baseline state involved low levels of activity spread evenly across the brain gave way to the discovery
that
a certain set of “default mode” brain regions were actually more active during rest than during task
engagement.
99101 Some of the key regions of the default mode network include portions of the anterior
medial
prefrontal cortex, ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (PFC), posterior cingulate complex,
hippocampus,
and other medial temporal lobe structures, and lateral temporal and parietal areas. 99101
This
“default mode” is not an inert or “resting” baseline for psychological function. Although default
mode
areas often experience reduced overall activity during certain cognitive and attentional
performance
tasks, they do experience increased activity during social cognitive tasks. 102 Thus, the
brain
does not return to a stable functional baseline, at least at the spatiotemporal resolution of fMRI
data.
When a higher-order common factor is used to model the correlations between response inhibition,
working
memory updating, and mental set shifting, this common factor predicts the inhibition factor
almost
perfectly. However, there is significant variation in the updating and shifting factors that are
unrelated
to the common factor. Resting-state and functional connectivity data suggest several distinct
configurations
(networks) of PFC and other brain regions, the lateral fronto-parietal (“central
executive”)
network (FPN), anchored in the dorsolateral (dl) and dorsomedial PFC and posterior parietal
cortex;
the cingulo-opercular network (CON), which overlaps with a salience network and includes
the
ACC, insula, and subcortical regions; the “ventral attention” network, which includes the inferior
frontal
gyrus, regions of the insula, and the temporoparietal junction; and the “dorsal attention” network,
which
includes the frontal field and intraparietal sulcus; and the “default mode network” (DMN)
comprising
medial PFC regions that interact with certain posterior cortical regions. The brain
processes
three sources of motivation: extrinsic rewards, moral values, and image concerns. According
to
decision neuroscience, when choosing whether to accept or reject an offer that weighs two types of
attributes
(moral values and money), the brain assigns a value to each option and compares them by
calculating
their difference. This scheme has been applied successively in the field of value-based
decision
-making in relation to various types of benefits (money) and costs (waiting for a long delay).
A
neurobehavioral and cognitive pattern that shares the same brain activation mechanisms characterizes
the
primary NDE with an emotional survival function and the secondary NDE with a financial/physical
survival
function. Although it may seem like a split-second phenomenon, deliberating about which
pág. 7311
emotions
we perceive unfolds in several stages of decision-making processing. General neurocognitive
models
of perception postulate that our brain first extracts sensory information about the world, then
integrates
this data into a percept, and finally interprets it. Furthermore, the left amygdala was
responsive
across all classes of decision-making paradigms, regardless of task demands. Bilateral brain
processes
for nonverbal decisions, left brain processes for verbal decisions. The mechanistic explanation
of
emotion perception consists of constructivist theories, which argue that the emotions of others can be
accurately
inferred from a combination of perception of motor expression, contextual processing, and
conceptual
knowledge about the relationships between emotions, desires, and beliefs. Perception of
motor
expressions can inform the process of emotion inference not because motor expressions reflect
emotions
per se (there is no one-to-one mapping ), but because the observer has learned through
experience
to associate certain motor expressions with certain emotional experiences through a process
of
bootstrapping .
Perception

Our
perception of free will is composed of a desire to act (volition) and a sense of responsibility for our
actions
(agency). 89109 Lesions that disrupt agency also occur in many different locations, but they
fall
within a separate network defined by connectivity with the precuneus. 89109 Together, these
networks
may underlie our perception of free will, with implications for neuropsychiatric diseases in
which
these processes are impaired.89109 Recent research has therefore focused on understanding this
perception
by dividing it into two processes: the intention or motivation to act, termed volition, and the
sense
of responsibility for one’s own action, termed agency. 89109 This view has received considerable
empirical
support from studies showing that spatial and temporal discrepancies between performing an
action
and seeing visual feedback of the action reduce the sense that the observed action is one’s own.
89
109 Thus, introducing a spatial transformation between an action and its visual consequences reduces
participants
sense of agency in proportion to the induced mismatch. 89109 The so-called “intentional
binding”
effect provides another line of evidence for the role of temporal contiguity between action and
outcome
in the construction of agency. The intentional binding effect was first reported by Haggard
et
al. and refers to the subjective compression of the temporal interval between a voluntary action and
its
external sensory consequences. 89109 The intentional binding effect would constitute an implicit but
pág. 7312
reliable
measure of agency, since it only occurs when events in the external environment are accurately
recognized
as consequences of one’s action.89109 In comparative accounts, a positive sense of agency is
the
default operation when no mismatch occurs between anticipated and actual states. Thus, a reliable
and
explicit sense of agency can only be formed when reafferent signals (visual, motor, or
proprioceptive)
become available for matching with intentions. 89109 Thus, one cannot feel agency
over
any event until that event has been registered and processed in the brain. As a consequence, agency
can
only be attributed retrospectively , although it is informed by on-line signals about motor orientation
and
control. However, an alternative possibility is that the sense of agency is also generated
prospectively
, before the action itself and before the actual effect of the actions is known. The onset
of
a random-dot visual stimulus marks the beginning of the decision process. The useful information is
encoded
by a set of neurons in the visual cortex . The flow of information from the stimulus is processed
by
specific regions in the visual cortex, which provide a stream of evidence for downstream processes.
89
109 The stream of momentary evidence comes from neurons in the visual cortex, concentrated in the
middle
temporal (MT) brain region. Other neurons, residing in the association cortex, represent the
accumulation
of this momentary evidence. 89109 A fundamental property of neurons in these areas
the
vast majority of the cortical mantle in primates is their ability to sustain firing for long periods in
the
absence of immediate sensory input or immediate motor effect. While MT neurons are spiking at
a
roughly constant rate, neurons in the LIP gradually increase or decrease their firing rate as more
evidence
accumulates for or against one of the choices. 89109 If the stimulus is turned off and a delay
period
occurs, MT neurons return to their baseline firing rates, but LIP neurons whose response fields
contain
the chosen target emit a sustained discharge that indicates the outcome of the decision.
However,
the firing pattern of neurons representing momentary evidence is entirely caused by input
from
other neurons, many of which are already in a continuum of activity and others are dormant.”
The
prefrontal cortex plays an important role not only in determining which events to perceive but also
which
of the corresponding response dispositions to select and actualize in overt behavior. 89109 Thus,
action
selection is the result of competition between response tendencies in the context of prefrontal bias
signals
that represent goal drives and strivings. 89109 Action selection can be decoupled from drives and
strivings
as a result of a lowering of the threshold for action selection. The emotional inhibition task
pág. 7313
engaged
the OFC and amygdala, whereas a think-no-think task engaged the hippocampus, consistent
with
work by Anderson, who demonstrated that memory retrieval can be inhibited by a dlPFC- to-
hippocampal
pathway via relays in the mediotemporal lobe or retrosplenial cortex. 89109 The term
categorical
perception describes the subjective experience in which a perceived dimension jumps
abruptly
from one category to another at a certain point along a continuum, rather than changing
gradually.
89109 Categorical and continuous perception often occur simultaneously when processing
the
emotions of others: the former allows for a gestalt perception of a single emotion, while the latter
allows
us to perceive subtle variations within an emotional construct. 89109 However, categorical
perception
seems to dominate the way we process and attribute emotions in others, and the reason for
this
may be to achieve cognitive efficiency by parsing information into meaningful but limited chunks
of
information. The influence of context and language also increases in the inference of emotional
state,
because the richer the emotional expression in situational information (a booming laugh), the less
need
there is to rely on context to infer the respective emotional state. To date, less attention has been
paid
to how higher cognitive functions (language processes, access to semantic knowledge) contribute
to
the formation of holistic perception and its interpretation in various contexts. Perception occurs when
incoming
sensory information is made available to higher-order brain regions and compared to a mental
model.
In the ventral stream, this mental model consists of semantic categorical representations, such
as
a prototype of a stimulus, or how a familiar face involuntarily stimulates. In the dorsal stream, the
mental
model consists of visuo- optic and audio-motor sequences potentially stored in our procedural
memory,
demonstrating how emotional expressions and emotional statements evolve over time. These
models
allow us to perceive and discriminate the actions of other individuals, including facial
movements
and speech. A later stage in the process of emotional perception concerns emotional
categorization
or verbal labeling. By converting the set of sensory information into a percept that can
be
communicated, an individual is able to relate to and describe the emotional state of another individual.
In
relation to this stage, a significant task-dependent role is assigned to the frontal cortex in matching
incoming
sensory information with a mental model. Although there are reviews and meta-analyses on
the
multiple roles of the inferior frontal cortex (IFC) in the perception of a large class of stimuli, there
are
no systematic reviews for the perception of emotional expressions, despite abundant empirical
pág. 7314
data.
.103105 Another frontal brain structure consistently recruited during perceptual decisions about
emotions
is the dorsomedial frontal cortex ( dmFC ). .103105 This structure is predominantly involved
in
social cognition, such as forming impressions about others and inferring beliefs, desires , and
intentions.
Emotional expressions conveyed by others are inherently social stimuli and are processed
differently
than other classes of stimuli, such as inanimate objects. The involvement of the dmFC in
this
specific decision-making process suggests that facial, bodily, and vocal emotional expressions are
not
only proxies for mental states, but also that observers spontaneously infer mental traits and states
(beliefs,
desire, intention) that are integrated into emotional appraisal.103105 In addition to the frontal
brain
structures targeted by the ventral and dorsal processing streams, other brain structures that do not
belong
exclusive leither processing stream also contribute to perceptual decisions about emotions.103105
One
such structure is the amygdala, which works concurrently with sensory and higher-order cortices
to
tag incoming sensory information with contextual relevance and subsequently detect this relevance
upon
the next encounter with that stimulus.103105 The fear conditioning procedure instills an initially
neutral
stimulus with the aim of eliciting biologically relevant responses and behaviors (freezing or
fleeing)
upon consistent association with an unconditioned aversive stimulus. 89109 Furthermore, the
amygdala
likely processes various emotional expressions, such as facial expressions and vocal prosody
features,
as relevant social cues. The amygdala may serve as one of the interfaces between sensory
cortices
and higher-order brain structures. 89109 The overwhelming evidence suggests that the right
pSTS
is involved in decoding and understanding meaningful social actions conveyed by gaze direction,
body
movement, and other types of meaningful goal-directed movement or implied by spoken words.
89
109
Neurodysfunctional
Attention
Attention
plays an essential role in motivating behavior and cognition, such as language and memory,
and
selectively enhances the processing of relevant sensory information Attention is our ability to
focus
on relevant information rather than irrelevant information. Attention has qualities and intensities.
When
focusing on a task, attention can be distracted by new, unexpected, but task-irrelevant events. This
is
the case with distracting sounds, which impair performance in children, as they are part of the long-
term
development of attention control. The ability to focus attention and ignore novel sounds that are
pág. 7315
irrelevant
to the task develops considerably from age 4 to 6. Different sources of motivation contribute
to
the regulation of sensory processing in the cortex and perceptual behavior , such as goal-directed
attention
to cognitively demanding tasks, which is often driven by external incentives. Attention levels
can
also be heightened by an internal desire to complete a task without any apparent change in external
incentives,
but it does require cognitive effort. The ability to act on the basis of the current incentive
value
of the outcomes of the action is a defining characteristic of purposeful or goal-directed behavior,
distinguishing
goal- directed responses from stimulus-elicited habits that are unaffected by changes in
outcome
values. Some studies show that V4 neuronal activity in the visual cortex is increased as a
result
of increased cognitive effort in response to increased attentional task demands . Poort et al.
demonstrated
that distinct mechanisms of response suppression are carried out by inhibitory and
excitatory
neurons in the visual cortex, associated with top-down input models, and thus generate
improved
sensory processing due to learning and attention. .103-105 The social brain in a neuromaladaptive
state
promotes dysfunctional inattention, with the aim of avoiding threats of emotional distress and
indirectly
generates effects of neglect and low diligence processes. .103-105 The term “unity and diversity”
(UD)
is used by neuroscientists to describe the relationships between these diverse frontal lobe
processes,
responsible for an “irrational variety” in human reactions, even with restricted and non-
progressive
lesions of the prefrontal regions, characterized as “compulsivity” or “abnormally stimulus-
linked
behavior”. The common and basic element of UD is “neglect of goal or disregard for a known
task
requirement”. .103-105 The study of the PFC is a great ally in deducing the organization of the
mediation
of the variety of cognitive processes, such as the dominance of automatic or dominant
responses,
interference controls, task alternations and coordination, updating of working memory,
monitoring
and planning. Social cognition is the process that guides behaviors towards other individuals
of
the same species. Several brain structures play a key role in controlling social behaviors: the
ventromedial
prefrontal cortex, the amygdala, the right somatosensory cortex and the insula. What
we
pay attention to is influenced by the reward of learning. Humans automatically attend to stimuli
previously
associated with reward and to stimuli that have been experienced and conditioned during
visual
search, even when it is disadvantageous in current situations. Several studies have shown that
associative
reward learning alters the brain's processing mechanisms of visual stimuli, in the face of
pág. 7316
learned
reward cues that are difficult to ignore. .103-105 The neuroanatomical connectivity of the PFC
associated
with most cortical and subcortical regions mediates several neural networks, and thus
generates
cognitive states of control and attention in different functional domains (spatial, visual and
verbal).
.103-105 However, changes in mental state require several subprocesses, such as interference
control,
recovery and reconfiguration of task sets. Changes due to the influence of previously learned
stimulus
-reward associations, but which can currently be considered irrelevant, is a phenomenon called
"value
-driven attentional capture" (VDAC)..103-105 The VDAC are representations of stimulus
associated
with reward undergo plasticity in the sensory cortex, automatically capturing attention during
the
initial processing of any lived experience. .103-105 Behavior directed toward goals or associated
outcomes
presents motivational elements based on cognitive values , which influence decision making
and
aid in interpretation in order to distinguish motivational control from value-based processes..103-105
Studies
of selective attention typically consider the role of task goals, but attention can also be captured
by
stimuli previously associated with reward or emotional defense. 103-105 Many studies show that
reward
learning induces visual cortical plasticity, which modulates early visual processing to capture
attention;
learned value modulates spatial signals in visual cortical areas, an effect that correlates with
VDAC.
.103-105 Attentional capture by previously rewarded targets as the modulatory effect of reward
on
priming , as well as the decoupling of reward history and prior task relevance in value-driven
attention.
Studies using magnetic encephalography have investigated modulations by reward learning,
and
have shown that VDAC is supported by learned value signals that modulate spatial selection
throughout
the posterior visual and parietal cortex, which can still occur in the absence of changes in
visual
processing in the cortex. .103-105 This value modulation is influenced by the strength of the
behavioral
VDAC effect and persists in subsequent target processing. Recent studies have demonstrated
that
VDAC is based on Pavlovian conditioning, and behavioral evidence distinguishes VDAC from
other
established control mechanisms, suggesting a distinct underlying neurobiological process. .103105
The
VDAC persists perennially even without reinforcement, unlike other forms of learning, where
removal
of reinforcement typically leads to extinction. .103105 Evidence suggests that the time course of
resource
and reward is flexible, with some constraint on learning the resourcereward association .
When
these value-cuing stimuli appeared as distractors in the test phase, they continuously shaped
pág. 7317
attentional
selection despite their task irrelevance. .103105 Rule-guided allocation of attention to
different
stimulus dimensions produced discriminate patterns of activation in visual cortex, providing a
signature
of top-down bias in perception. Survival in a changing environment requires qualities that
predict
social reality. Our reasoning seeks patterns of information that provide predictions. The brain
tries
to make it easier for itself to predict the behavior of others without communication, since
coordinated
repetitive movements of closely related individuals are more predictable to the brain.
Cognitive
development and social cognition require individuals to connect with and coordinate with
relatives
(and/or peers) to capture patterns of information that provide predictions. The differentiation
of
facial expressions is a process of increased mental activity, which has considerable applications in
the
clinical setting of emotions, as attentional bias toward threat is implicated in the etiology and
maintenance
of anxiety disorders. 103-105 In The fMRI studies of adults exposed to familiar and
unfamiliar
videos, it was shown that neuroadaptations led to tighter connectivity between the ACC and
the
anterior insula, consolidating an interface combining information from exteroceptive and
interoceptive
sources to support attachment representations. Viewing one's own child versus a familiar
child
was associated with activation in the amygdala, insula, anterior paracingular cortex, and posterior
superior
temporal sulcus Viewing. The face of an unfamiliar child compared with that of an unfamiliar
adult
engages areas associated with attention as well as face perception. In response to pictures of
children,
parents also exhibit higher plasma oxytocin concentrations, as well as stronger activation in
brain
regions important for decoding facial emotions (caudal middle frontal gyrus), mentalizing
(temporoparietal
junction), and reward processing (medial orbitofrontal cortex). In summary,
socioemotional
studies show a mixed pattern of affected functions; fMRI and neural hodology studies
have
shown connectivity dysfunctions in the amygdala and parietal networks, aberrant frontoparietal
and
temporoposterior networks with relative preservation of the posterior part of the default mode
network
and the visual network. They also showed a significant disconnection in the frontal pathways.
Several
neuroscientists have shown that value signals automatically guide attention to new situations
and
associations, which may or may not be beneficial, depending on their congruence with current goals.
Cognitive
control allows stimulus-response processing to be aligned with internal goals and is therefore
central
to intelligent and purposeful behavior. Control depends on the active representation of task
pág. 7318
information
in the prefrontal cortex (PFC), which provides a source of contextual bias in perception,
decision
-making, and action. Studies that analyzed the strength of neuronal connectivity in the PFC
have
shown that the inferior frontal sulcus and intraparietal sulcus during context-dependent decision-
making
are functionally active at rest and during task performance. Attentional focusing ensures that
emotionally
salient features of complex events are preferentially retained in memory, which may be an
evolutionary
disadvantage. The superior colliculus ( SC) dominates visual behavior during the first
months
of life, is active at birth for complex visual tasks, and has a significant influence on several
hemispheric
regions. The SC is the main brain center, with a subcortical structure, that permanently
integrates
visual and nonvisual , external and internal (bottom-up and top-down, respectively)
information,
and has the enigmatic ability to make nonconscious decisions about where to focus
attention.
It is also a sentinel that triggers the subcortical mechanisms that drive social motivation to
follow
faces from birth and automatically respond to emotional stimuli. 99-102 Through indirect
connections,
it also simultaneously activates several cortical structures necessary for the development
of
social cognition and the multiattentional task required for conscious social interaction in real-life
scenarios.
It also plays a crucial role in the integration of external and internal senses with emotional,
autonomic,
and endocrine functions, as well as in visual/motor transformation, target selection, and goal-
directed
motor responses. 99-102 Its multiple direct connections from the cortex are responsible for the
shift
from covert to overt attention and the interaction of medial and lateral cortical brain networks,
which
regulate and integrate endogenous attention with external attention, respectively. .103-105 With
the
description of the Sprague effect , a hemineglect syndrome produced by a unilateral lesion of the
SC,
or the resolution of hemineglect in a patient with a frontal lesion, after inactivation of the
contralateral
SC, evidence was given to the significant influence of the SC on hemispheric functions.
In
blindsight, the primary visual cortex is compromised, but the SC and the magnocellular visual
pathway
are spared and the individual is able to react to stimuli.99-102 This is evidence of the role of
the
SC in ongoing, multi-attentional, non-conscious emotional processing, including autonomic and
motor
reactions. Affective and motivational aspects of visual attention and saccadic control activate
the
“Emotional System” or “Alarm System” of the SC, and thus simultaneously trigger attentional,
autonomic,
endocrine, and cognitive functions via the dopaminergic reward system , the adrenergic alert
pág. 7319
system,
the cholinergic system, and the endocrine system . 99-102 Abstraction is a intellectual operation
which
consists of isolating, in a concept , an element to the exclusion of others, from which abstraction
is
then made. The notion of abstraction is important for understanding some philosophical
controversies
in relation to empiricism, universalism and evidence-based medicine. Abstraction is the
cognitive
operation through which something is chosen as an object of perception, attention,
observation,
consideration, research, study, judgment and others. It is isolated from other things with
which
it is in a greater relationship. Spatial cognition corresponds to the ability of an individual to
perceive
the spatial relationships between objects as well as to present themselves with the notions of
depth,
solidity and distance. The Heteromodal Association Area of the Posterior Parietal Cortex is
important
because it is associated with different syndromes that significantly involve deficits in spatial
cognition.
99-102 The main sensory pathways (visual, tactile, proprioceptive, auditory and vestibular)
that
receive stimuli for the development of the spatial cognition process. A spatial fixation point is the
site
of conscious attention, where we usually focus attention; where we fixate, it is possible to allocate
some
attention away from the fixation point (covert attention).99-102 This covert attention is known to
shift
or compress receptive visual fields toward the attended location, to improve visual acuity at the
locus
of attention, although covert attention also defines a center of reference that affects the visuomotor
response.
99-102 This cognitive ability is closely correlated with spatial perception, which is formed by
the
end result of the organization and integration of diverse sensory stimuli, which aim to assist in the
comprehension
of consciousness in a general panorama of the relationships and forms of the external
environment.
.103-105
Neuroesthetics

Perhaps
it is more appropriate to say that beauty is in each sense of the perceiver. The experience of the
world
is primarily multisensory and integrated across different sensory modalities, so many of our
decisions
are based on aesthetics as perceived by multiple sensory modalities rather than a single sensory
modality.
Aesthetics can refer only to external, or material, design, as if a food looks good but does
not
taste good, most of us will not choose to eat that food, and we will not buy a scent just by seeing its
color,
but by smelling it as well. Thus, multisensory cues can, separately or in combination, influence
our
perceived attractiveness or aesthetics of an individual or object, and our attitudes and actions toward
pág. 7320
that
person or object. It is therefore important to deepen our understanding of how the human aesthetic
process
operates across sensory modalities and how this process is distinct from basic perceptual
processing.
Karim AKMR et al ., present a model based on hierarchical information processing styles,
disentangling
aesthetic processing from basic perceptual processing, and demonstrate how affective and
cognitive
influences interact to modulate aesthetic preferences under both top-down and bottom-up
control.
In support of this model, we present findings from cognitive neuroscience, neuroaesthetics,
affective
science, psychology, and the arts that highlight the crucial role that different cortical regions
play
in recognizing objects or stimuli and appreciating their beauty in visual and nonvisual modalities,
and
how their roles may be mediated by experience. Basic perception depends on the explicit
properties
of the stimulus and the perceptibility of the cognitive agent, while aesthetic perception or
appreciation
may or may not depend on the explicit properties of the stimulus, but from the individual
characteristics
of the observer's cognitive agent. Personal characteristics of the cognitive agent that may
further
shape aesthetic preference include culture, experience, interest , aesthetic mindset, emotional
state,
or motivation. These arise from the dynamic interaction between the cognitive agent and the
object,
rather than just explicit "objective" properties of the object or "subjective" characteristics of the
cognitive
agent. A projected image may not be appreciated by a particular religious community because
people
in that community believe that there is no place for images, and thus it goes against religious
code,
belief systems, and value, and so is perceived as unaesthetic and unbeautiful, suggesting that
beauty
depends on value and is in the eye of the beholder. 99-102 Thus, the perceived quality of an
object
reflects the observer's opinion or judgment about its quality ( aesthetics) independent of its actual
physical
qualities, and thus an unfair opinion or judgment may arise. An aesthetic property is
distinguished
from a descriptive or basic physical property in that the perception of an aesthetic property
involves
cognitive evaluation, hedonic valuation, and often knowledge-dependent concepts, but the
perception
of a descriptive physical property does not. 99102 A descriptive property, such as being
rectangular
or being red, can be attributed without any belief about its evaluation and hedonic status,
whether
positive, negative, or neutral, are natural attributes. 99102 The perception of the descriptive
property
(being a circle, a triangle, or a square; lexical status of letter strings) can operate with or without
consciousness,
or attention, but the perception of the aesthetic property does not. 99102 Thus, basic
pág. 7321
perception
is a non-evaluative form of cognitive process or a purely non-cognitive process that generally
does
not induce any emotional feelings, whereas aesthetic perception or appreciation is not only a
definite
cognitive evaluative process , but also induces emotional feelings). However, a complexity
associated
with aesthetic perception is the conflicting aesthetic emotions elicited by the composition of
multifaceted
stimuli. The coexistence of aesthetic and non-aesthetic properties in the same stimulus is
likely
to induce simultaneously positive and negative emotions in the cognitive agent. In such an
approach
- avoidance dilemma , aesthetic preference may be determined by the resulting impact of the
two
opposites on the elicitation of aesthetic emotions.
Automatisms
and cognitive control Automatic information processing is not a unitary construct, but
rather
diagnosed considering the presence of different characteristics such as uncontrollable, efficient,
unconscious,
and sudden. The characteristics of automaticity are not intrinsic to the processes , but
point
to the conditions under which the neural processes operate or to the variables that influence their
occurrence.
Psychological processes should not be diagnosed as automatic or non-automatic in a
categorical
way, but rather as more or less automatic than other processes, depending on the number
and
extent of the characteristics. Distraction is a functional strategy of emotional regulation used to
alleviate
a state of discomfort or emotional distress. Within the perspective of the attention economy,
distraction
is increasingly associated with the use of digital technology, performance deficiencies , and
interference
with higher-order cognitive processes. Distraction can be conceptualized as the result of a
reaction
to exogenous (orientation system) or endogenous (alert system) signals, or as the result of a
conflict
between these two networks that are competing for attentional resources. Endogenous signals
(alert
system) are the bottom-up signals in the form of expectations, worries, and persistent thoughts
that
lead to distraction or daydreaming. The executive system is implicated when a conflict arises
between
exogenous and endogenous signals, leading to discontinuity of attention and, therefore, to poor
deployment
of attention, causing difficulties in inhibitory or executive control, which are dependent on
dopaminergic
neurons. Cognitive control (CC) is a term generally associated with the healthy
functioning
of the PFC and its related regions, such as the cingulate cortex, and is considered
synonymous
with the earlier notion of executive function (EF). 89-95 It also involves a central process
of
behavioral regulation that optimizes its goal-directedness and counteracts automaticity. 89-95 The CC
pág. 7322
allows
stimulus-response processing to be aligned with internal and external goals, which is essential
for
conscious (purposeful), intelligent, and rational action. 89-95 The role of the PFC in the CC consists
essentially
of contextually biasing attention (through instructions) to resolve conflicts and exert
attentional
control. 89-95 Several fMRI studies show that ACC activation was accompanied by activation
of
the dorsolateral (dl) PFC associated with top-down adjustments of response control, which can be
termed
a “multiple demand” (MD) system. 89-95 The cellular physiology of these regions is
characterized
by rapid firing of neurotransmitters such as dopamine, and they present neural plasticity
properties
that allow functions such as goal maintenance in working memory. 89-95 The three functional
abilities
of the CC are inhibition of a prepotent response (stopping an automatic response, sometimes to
make
an alternative response), updating working memory (continually replacing no longer relevant
information
in working memory with newly relevant information as it is detected in the environment),
and
switching mental sets (alternating between two alternative tasks). The CC operates in different
internal
and external states, such as stress, which alters the neurochemical environment, and the
“inverted
U-shaped” functioning of the PFC, which translates into job performance effectiveness and
general
intelligence. Individual differences in response inhibition tasks may be particularly related to
this
ability, because if a goal is inactive or ineffective, more automatic or prepotent responses will take
over,
leading to poor performance on these tasks. 99102 They characterized the unity of frontal lobe
functions
in terms of goal-related processes, specifically the ability to form and realize goals at multiple
levels
of abstraction . 8995 Studies have shown that goal neglect was more closely related to general
brain
atrophy than to focal frontal lesions , and that these general goal-related processes are related to
the
MD network, a network of frontal and parietal regions that are commonly activated across tasks. 89
95
Several studies have demonstrated moderate to large correlations between measures of intelligence,
particularly
measures of fluid intelligence (such as reasoning), and measures related to the CC. 89-95
Hybrid
organizational systems may incorporate features of MD in certain PFC regions, but also allow
for
specificity of neural connectivity to mediate specific aspects of response inhibition, updating, and
cognitive
flexibility. 89-95 In the case of memory of work , the lateral frontal cortex in primates, already
implicated
in the spatial delayed response task, was known to contain cells that exhibited activity in
delay
periods in response to stimuli in several sensory modalities. 8995 Electrophysiological evidence,
pág. 7323
as
well as human functional imaging, suggests that the inferotemporal/perirhinal and anterior parietal
cortex
also exhibited maintenance operations, although the dlPFC appeared to have important roles in
resisting
interference (distraction) in working memory. Recent studies have shown the role of posture
in
alertness and found evidence for top-down executive control in the maintenance of alertness. Alerting
has
been linked to the norepinephrine system that modulates frontal and parietal structures. The
executive
network is involved in error detection, conflict resolution, and other aspects of performance,
and
involves the anterior cingulate and anterior insula (operculum). 89-95 Many psychiatric and
neurological
disorders are associated with specific symptoms that may be at least in part a product of
impaired
CC, or with more general cognitive deficits that, when accompanied by emotional behaviors,
may
express prolonged neurocognitive states and moments of moral dementia.” Many theories and
debates
about human intelligence have presented theories as single regions, or overlapping specific
networks
, but the discoveries of neural hodology and the reconnection of interhemispheric neurons may
help
to better understand. The emphasis on whole-brain multinetwork connectivity, and the inclusion
of
dysfunctions of weak connections in some regions and strong connections in others, motivates the
prediction
that intelligence will depend on functional connections that are distributed globally
throughout
the connectome. Using resting-state fMRI data , we expand our understanding of the
neurobiological
substrates that give rise to intelligence and emphasize the importance of considering
neural
representations of lower activity in emotional and maladaptive neurosystems.
Alexithymia
and Anosognosia
According
to Taylor et al., alexithymia is a personality trait essentially characterized by deficits in the
ability
to identify and describe emotions and reduced tendencies to engage in fantasies and reflect on
emotional
experiences. 89-95 The term alexithymia, which literally means “lack of words for emotion”,
is
thus an identification of a neurological state based on clinical observations in many patients with
psychosomatic
illnesses, who have a restricted affective life and hardly benefit from psychodynamic
therapy.
Some authors define it as a personality trait characterized by difficulties in recognizing
(identifying)
and then accurately verbalizing one’s own emotions, using a cognitive style oriented
towards
external events, rather than intrapsychic experiences. 89-95 Attention predominates in the
awareness
of external reality and thus presents difficulties in identifying feelings, deficits in automatic
pág. 7324
facial
expressions in neuroadaptation states. The neuropsychological state of alexithymia is associated
with
several psychiatric disorders, such as depression, anxiety, and eating disorders , and is therefore
considered
a general vulnerability factor for the development of mental disorders. Furthermore, it seems
likely
that alexithymic individuals receive negative feedback from the social environment about their
deficits
in perceiving, feeling, and communicating emotions and their superficial ways of thinking. 89-95
Nemiah
presented a deficit model in which a reduced capacity for fantasy formation and experience of
feelings
is proposed, and a deficient representation of feelings and a diminished affective response are
assumed.
Different neurobiological models have been proposed to explain the etiology of alexithymia.
suggested
that inadequate neural connections between limbic and neocortical areas may be a
contributing
factor to the development of alexithymia. According to Buchanan et al., since a right
hemisphere
advantage has been described in the perception of emotion, it has been hypothesized that
alexithymia
could reflect disturbances in the functioning of the right hemisphere. A popular etiological
model
of alexithymia represents the interhemispheric transfer deficit or functional commissurotomy
model
according to which emotional material is disconnected from the left hemisphere when verbally
expressive
situations occur. According to the hypoarousal model, alexithymic individuals are
characterized
by dampened sympathetic nervous system activation in response to affective stimuli.
Individuals
who spontaneously develop stronger physiological responses to emotional stimuli should
become
more quickly aware of them and also make them more accessible to conscious reflection.
Alexithymic
individuals show low reactivity to negative emotional stimuli, in which the brain regions
responsible
for evaluation, encoding and affective response, such as the occipitotemporal areas,
amygdala
and insula, work less, and are characterized simultaneously with anosognosia with the same
deficits
in perceiving others, by automatic activation again of the alexithymic circuits. The fMRI
studies
in alexithymic individuals have demonstrated poor states of feelings, few affective responses
and
deficits in emotional reactivity. Alexithymic individuals suffer in interpersonal relationships due
to
deficits in expressing themselves, feeling, perceiving, communicating, in front of "normal" people
who
also do not perceive, the real problem of alexithymics. Alexithymic individuals show low
reactivity
to negative emotional stimuli, in which the brain regions responsible for evaluation, encoding
and
affective response, such as the occipitotemporal areas, amygdala and insula, work less . fMRI
pág. 7325
studies
in alexithymics have demonstrated poor states of feelings, few affective responses and deficit of
emotional
reactivity. These are dysfunctions of neuronal connection, between limbic and neocortical
areas.
The reduced response of the fusiform gyrus to facial emotions with The poor physical
perception
in alexithymic individuals may affect the brain's visual processing, and thus impair the
recognition
of emotional facial expressions of others. This process is currently classified by modern
neuroscience
as anosognosia. fMRI studies in alexithymic patients and masked individuals have
observed
fewer harsh responses and have shown little amygdala activation to unidentifiable faces, with
or
without threat. Alexithymic features may be consequences of deficits in interhemispheric
communication,
low limbic-neocortical connectivity and/or right hemisphere dysfunction and excessive
amygdala
activity. It is conceivable that alexithymic features phenomenologically represent a kind of
final
common pathway to the automatic processing deficits in alexithymia. Several studies show that
children
who are maltreated show deficits in the recognition, expression and understanding of emotions.
Higher
levels of maltreatment in childhood or adolescence have been associated with higher levels of
alexithymia.
Researchers suggest its presence as a transdiagnostic mechanism . Other pathological
situations
of alexithymia are when patients present with schizophrenia, psychopathy, autism, acute
neurointoxication
of substances, neurodysfunctions and long-lasting epigenetics, such as relapse states,
allostatic
cycles, pathological demotivation. Duan et al., showed negative associations between the
subscale
difficulties in identifying feelings and activity in response to masked facial surprise, in the
fusiform,
parahippocampal and superior temporal gyri. Pollatos et al., presented results of difficulties
in
identifying and describing feelings, associated with smaller electrodermal responses to briefly
presented
negative images (but not to positive or neutral images). Sonnby-Borgström examined facial
electromyographic
(EMG) activity of the corrugator and zygomaticus muscles in response to masked
(and
unmasked) emotional facial expressions in patients with alexithymia, and found that individuals
with
severe alexithymia showed less corrugator muscle imitation in response to masked stimuli (and
lower
levels of automatic processing). Reduced activation in this network in response to emotional
stimuli
, as observed in alexithymia, may impede the effortless and unintentional computation of their
affective
value. Although most of the available evidence from neurobiological priming studies relates
to
visual emotional stimuli, there are also early indications that rapid and unintentional processing of
pág. 7326
auditory
emotional stimuli may be impaired in alexithymia. However, it should be noted that not all
findings
from neurobiological studies on automatic emotion processing and alexithymia suggest that
alexithymia
is related to brain hyporesponsiveness during emotion processing. In particular, the results
of
riau et al. demonstrate that activation of the anterior cingulate cortex to task- irrelevant threatening
faces
is increased in alexithymia. Van der Velde et al. have argued that the increased response of the
anterior
cingulate gyrus to emotional stimuli in alexithymic individuals may reflect increased cognitive
demands
to attend to and understand the emotional value of stimuli. Alexithymic features may be
consequences
of deficits in interhemispheric communication, low limbic-neocortical connectivity,
and/or
right hemisphere dysfunction. Although emotions are generally elicited involuntarily and arise
without
conscious effort, it is surprising that little attention in the etiological considerations of
alexithymia
has been given to deficits in automatic emotion processing and their neurobiological
underpinnings
.
Memory

Over
the past decade, advances in cognitive neuroscience have begun to unravel the biological mysteries
surrounding
the persistence of emotional experiences in humans, with implications for understanding
memory
disorders in affective disorders. According to Josselyn SA et al., memory is the ability to use
the
past in the service of the present or future; it is central to our daily lives and defines who we are;
without
it, we are condemned to an eternal present. Episodic memories are encoded by neuronal
ensembles
activated by experience that remain necessary and sufficient for recall. Emotion has
significant
influences on learning and memory that involve several brain systems at different stages of
information
processing. They are influenced by arousal and valence of the amygdala, and its interactions
with
the frontal and temporal lobes. Currently, studies present accumulated data on the effects of
emotion
on memory systems, such as declarative memory (events, episodic, explicit) and non-
declarative
memory ( implicit, fear conditioning). Recognition memory has two dissociable processes:
familiarity
discrimination and recollection. Familiarity discrimination for individual visual stimuli is
performed
in the perirhinal cortex of the temporal lobe. A systematic review has shown that the number
of
memories stored by a familiarity detection network depends on the rule of synaptic plasticity (storage
depends
on processes that produce synaptic weakening). Neural signals of visual familiarity were
pág. 7327
observed
as reductions in responses to repeated presentations of a stimulus, a phenomenon known as
repetition
suppression. Memory for a stimulus is degraded in two ways: plasticity events obscure
existing
memories, and plastic weights weaken over time. Memory-selective neurons prefer either novel
or
familiar stimuli, and scale response as a function of confidence. They signal subjective choices,
independent
of truth and values. Confidence-selective neurons signal confidence, independent of
stimulus
familiarity. Together, this functional separation reveals the action-independent encoding of
familiarity
based on declarative memory and confidence in individual choices. Currently, multisensory
neuroscience
research uses clinical stimuli to investigate the impact of their content on intermodal
integration,
and their functional variations are translated by the term "semantic" . In several studies,
most
tasks that influence semantic factors were found without cortical networks that are more likely to
mediate
these effects. Cortical regions are particularly responsive to variations in content and states
of
semantic correspondences ("congruent") and incompatibilities ("incongruent"). Recent fMRI studies
point
to functional differentiation of temporal and frontal cortical regions, the former being more
responsive
to semantically congruent stimulation and the latter to semantically incongruent audiovisual
stimulation.
Doehrmann and Naumer, showed that in "normal " individuals with angry faces, they were
associated
with increased processing and effective connectivity of the inferior occipital regions, in
relation
to the ventrolateral prefrontal regions , and thus transmit disapproval with subsequent evocation
of
excessive fear responses and negative cognitions.
Emotional
episodic memory
The
memory-enhancing effects of emotional arousal involve interactions between subcortical and
cortical
structures and engagement of central and peripheral neurohormonal systems that are coordinated
by
the amygdala but limit the range of consciousness. Memory enhancement by arousal involves
similar
brain systems in positive and negative valences. However, the retention advantages of
emotional
valence in the absence of high arousal partially reflect semantic and strategic processes that
are
regulated in the frontal lobe and that benefit declarative memory without central amygdala
involvement.
The contributions of the amygdala, PFC, and MTL memory systems extend beyond the
initial
period of memory consolidation to initiate the retrieval of emotional memories, including
especially
those from the personal and intimate past . According to Hamann, autobiographical memory
pág. 7328
research
allows ethical evaluations of more intense and remote emotional episodes, as well as
investigations
of emotional influences on the phenomenology of memory but not on memory accuracy.
The
exact time course of consolidation is subject to considerable debate, and the stabilization of memory
traces
is a lengthy process that can last months to years. Emotional arousal has complementary and
immediate
effects during encoding that are time- invariant and are interpreted to reflect attentional
influences
on memory. Recent studies have demonstrated the retrieval of items from emotional contexts
with
activation of limbic structures (amygdala, insula, and cingulate) and several regions of the temporal
and
frontal neocortex. Studies of retrograde amnesia support. Markowitsch 's proposal that retrieval
of
remote personal memories involves interactions between the inferior PFC and its connections with
the
anteromedial temporal lobe that courses through the uncinate fasciculus. Furthermore, fMRI
studies
that analyzed autobiographical retrieval in healthy adults demonstrated activity in the
frontotemporal
regions, as well as in other regions, such as the medial PFC, retrosplenial cortex, and
extrastriate
cortex, which perform self-referential processing and visuospatial images. Emotional
intensity
affects the perceptual and phenomenological properties of autobiographical memories, such as
the
degree to which the memory is revived upon retrieval, the vividness of the memory, and the narrative
details.
Understanding how emotion transforms the remembrance experience that accompanies
autobiographical
memories can advance knowledge about the complex and subjective characteristics of
emotional
memory. Surface beliefs are illusory or self-deceiving , in relation to a deep or concrete belief.
Contextual
memory
Contextual
learning is a critical component of episodic memory and important for living in any
environment.
Although we may be focusing on a specific event or person at the moment, many other
information
streams occur simultaneously around the target to initiate the formation of contextual
memory
or context.”. External components collected by sensory systems, such as visual, odor, sound,
and
touch information, contribute to the formation of spatial-contextual information. External
information
and internal states are used. Internal elements are collected through the emotional states,
hormonal
states, or stress experienced within the situation . The effects of internal states can predispose
a
circuit to be more or less responsive to a given cue. 7798 External components are the elements of the
environment
or an object. We can describe context as the specifics of a place that are not the place itself .
pág. 7329
77
98 Thus, external elements are easier to manipulate and are often used as the primary means of
modifying
contexts.77-98 According to Marks et al., context modulates decisions, recall processes, and
plays
an important role in the initial processing and reconstruction of episodic memories. It also aids in
the
agility of object representations, motivational determination, and evaluation of actions. 77-98 There
are
several tasks in which place memory can be included as part of a context-dependent memory
paradigm
, such as contextual fear conditioning (CFC). 77-98 The entorhinal cortex (EC), a major
contributor
to the functionality of the hippocampal circuit, plays a key role in contextual memory
processes.
Lesions in this region result in decreased contextual learning, but not in avoidance learning.
77
-98 Hippocampal injury disrupts the CFC in a time-sensitive manner, with recent memories being
deleted
by the lesion and long-term contextual memories being undamaged. 77-98
Conceptual
Memory
The
sstorage and retrieval are mediated by subpopulations of neurons that are therefore considered
cellular
engrams. Human memory makes associations between different concepts. 7798 According to
the
Hebbian postulate, connections between neurons with correlated activity patterns are strengthened,
while
connections between neurons whose activity patterns are clearly correlated are depressed or even
lost.
7798 This phenomenon increases the likelihood that neural activity patterns that occur during
encoding
will reoccur at later time points. Therefore, the subset of synapses between coactive neurons
can
also be considered a memory substrate, or a synaptic engram.7798 The episode “with my friend
in
this place” gave rise to an association between two existing concepts: before the trip (the episodic
event),
you already knew your friend (first concept) and had seen the place (second concept), but only
after
the trip do you associate these two concepts. Concepts are encoded in the human middle temporal
lobe
(MTL) by neurons called “concept cells” that respond selectively and invariably to stimuli
representing
a specific person or place . Each concept is thought to be represented by a set of concept
cells
that increase their firing rates simultaneously upon presentation of an appropriate stimulus. With
shared
neurons, activation of a first assembly (place) can also activate a second assembly (person).
First,
for the brain to function properly as a memory network, it must remain possible to remember the
two
associated concepts separately (place without its friend) rather than automatically both together.77
98
However, if concepts share many neurons, it becomes likely that the two memory items can no longer
pág. 7330
be
distinguished but are merged into a single, larger concept encoded by a larger number of active
neurons.
Shared concept cells can be visualized as an overlap between two memory engrams.
Associative
memory in recurrent networks, such as area CA3 of the hippocampus, has been modeled
with
attractor neural networks where each memory item is encoded as a memory engram in a fixed
random
subset of neurons “pattern” , so that no pattern has an overlap above chance with another.
Association
chains can form the basis of a “stream of thought” where the direction of transitions from
one
concept to the next is based on learned associations7798 According to Romani-Tsodyks et al .,
memory
engrams are independent of finite-size effects and stimulate neuron sharing in some engram
pairs
above chance, which allows sequential retrieval in the presence of periodic background input. 77
98
Gastaldi C et al ., showed that in large sparse coding-level networks, neurons shared by chance are
not
sufficient to reliably induce the retrieval of a chain of concepts, since overlaps greater than chance,
represented
by associations learned experientially, are required. 77-98 Instead of transitions triggered by
oscillations,
transitions can also be triggered by two adaptation mechanisms that act on different time
scales
without the need for periodic inhibition. Attractor networks with sparse patterns and random
connectivity
are suitable candidate models for biological memory, due to two features: memory retrieval
after
stimulation with a partial cue and sustained activity after a stimulus has been removed.77-98
Objective

The
objective of this article is to assist in the resolution with the use of medical evaluation based on
neuroscience
and neurobiology, which were organized from clinical practice faithful to reality, with the
identification
of a phenomenon harmful to collective integral health, observed with methodology and
systematization
of accumulated data from scientific publications from the Pubmed digital library, all
selected
with clinical abstraction and thus convenience, when identifying a clinical and
pathophysio
logical causal link, which follows from a " continuum" of research after the initial
description
of Syndrome Z in 2022, Marília, interior of São Paulo, in a therapeutic community for drug
addicts
and from self-experienced experiences and empirical findings. In this work, we describe the
cognitive
and behavioral aspects that influence the quality of care and the effects on development in
contexts
of parental neglect, and in situations where there is unavailability of care resources.
pág. 7331
METHOD

We
present a clinical pre-diagnostic construct, based on neuro physiopathology, which was carried out
in
the practical field, and helps in the understanding and organized identification of a serious public
health
problem and a hidden setback in family courts. It aims to contribute to the problem of NP, and
its
medical and specialized characterization, as well as its already known social and psychological
consequences.
With concrete clinical updating and delimitation, with the organization of disorders
with
a clinical and physiopathological causal link, which present significant influence of genetic,
epigenetic,
neurodysfunctional, hormonal, inflammatory, immunological and metabolic orders.
RESULT

The
quality of care is directly reflected in maladaptive neurological development, with deficits in
emotional
regulation, intelligence and family and social skills, family synchrony and chronic states of
cognition
and behavior that distort the real values of mental health and the importance of interpersonal
relationships
( IR), in addition to influencing various aspects of stress in children and adolescents, and
have
a significant and direct influence on MD such as depression and anxiety and psychosomatic and
metabolic
disorders. The studies reviewed, together with the description of neurodysfunctional
behaviors
that interfere with affective attention, as other types of attention and values predominate,
which
are distorted in many cases for predominantly neurological reasons. Currently, there is no
attention
to this problem, and here, like other studies on Syndrome Z, they provide resources with real
chances
of prevention and improvement in various treatments of MD and chronic organic diseases that
have
an important neuropsychological component.
DISCUSSION

Instead
of applying psychological tests , we identify syndrome Z through clinical neuromarkers,
neurodysfunctional
behavioral and cognitive states, and amygdala automatisms, which are expressed by
alexithymia
and anosognosia, family schemas, chronic oscillatory hypodopaminergic state, which are
neurodevelopmental
responses in EAI environments and consequently NP in a subtle and hidden way.
In
addition to these momentary activation states, there is concomitant deficit of Mentalism or affective
attention,
which presents mechanisms dependent on neurobiology such as peripheral dopaminergic and
pág. 7332
oxytocin
neurons. And with the habit of not intervening in harmful behaviors, what is neuromaladaptive
and
even pathological becomes normal, and the normal and the merely adaptive are scarce and are not
identified,
with consonant concepts. The evidence shows that reality is not in the same direction as
current
legal laws, human rights, and objective medical ethics codes are moving in the opposite
direction,
even if the reference we should follow were civil laws, laws on children's rights, human rights,
and
codes of ethics. In Brazil, there is much research on the judicialization of Medicine, in which a
major
problem is medical error, which in many cases is due to professional negligence, or due to bodily
injury
or due to an ineffective doctor-patient relationship. Several pieces of evidence have shown that
complex
PTSD or a history of childhood abuse (or both), and even the presence of four or more
situations
of Adverse Childhood Emotions ( ACE), have a negative impact on parenting and on multiple
comorbidities,
physical and mental, and social risk behaviors, in addition to generating
"intergenerational
cycles" of trauma.
CONCLUSION

This
work described common and predominant states that occupy the space and time of emotional and
affective
attention, which is currently subtle and hidden from professionals and family members,
elucidating
a significant participation of neurobiological order, which always acts simultaneously with
subjective
or personal components, and experienced as experience, and begins to understand the danger
of
the rigidity of a pattern that is believed to be normal, where the normal does not present
neuromaladaptatio
n, but rather adaptation. This study should be reflected upon by professionals in
family
law and Family Medicine, as there is no assessment and much less protective care interventions
for
child neurodevelopment , which is sensitive to ACEs, which determine or limit the social, mental
and
organic life of the child.
BIBLIOGRA
PHIC REFERENCES
1.
Assis SG, Farias LOP, organizers. National survey of children and adolescents in institutional
care
services [Internet]. São Paulo: Hucitec Editora; 2013 [cited on 04 Jun 2018]. 367p. Available
at:

http://aplicacoes.mds.gov.br/sagi/dicivip_datain/ckfinder/userfiles/files/LIVRO_Levantamento

%20Nacional_Final.pdf
pág. 7333
2.
Kepple NJ, Parker A. Examining Unique Substance-related Risk Profiles for Neglectful
Behaviors
among Parents with and without Clinical Depression. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2021 Jun;
125:105987.
doi : 10.1016/j.childyouth.2021.105987. Epub 2021 Mar 15. PMID: 33986556;
PMCID:
PMC8112573.
3.
Crowell JA, Keluskar J, Gorecki A. Parenting behavior and the development of children with
autism
spectrum disorder. Compr Psychiatry. 2019 Apr; 90:21-29. doi :
10.1016/j.comppsych.2018.11.007.
Epub 2018 Nov 20. PMID: 30658339.
4.
Costa AP, Steffgen G, Vögele C. The role of alexithymia in parent-child interaction and in the
emotional
capacity of children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 2019 Mar; 12(3):458-
468.
doi : 10.1002/aur.2061. Epub 2019 Jan 9 . PMID: 30624024.
5.
Williams JH, Waiter GD, Gilchrist A, Perrett DI, Murray AD, Whiten A. Neural mechanisms of
imitation
and 'mirror neuron' function in autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychology. 2006;
44(4):610
-21. doi : 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.06.010. Epub 2005 Sep 2. PMID:
16140346.

6.
Simion F, Di Giorgio E, Leo I, Bardi L. The processing of social stimuli in early infancy: from
faces
to the perception of biological motion. ProgBrain Res. 2011;189:173-93. doi :
10.1016/B978
-0-444-53884-0.00024-5. PMID: 21489389.
7.
Rajkumar RP. Biomarkers of Neurodegeneration in Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder: An
Integrative
Review. Biomedicines. 2023 May 17; 11(5):1465. doi :
10.3390/biomedicines11051465.
PMID: 37239136; PMCID: PMC10216622.
8.
McCauley DM, Sloan CJ, Xia M, Fosco GM. Same family, divergent realities: How triangulation
preserves
parents' illusory harmony while adolescents navigate interparental conflicts. J Fam
Psychol.
2021 Mar; 35(2):128-137. doi : 10.1037/fam0000785. PMID: 33871274; PMCID:
PMC8336947.

9.
Rouse H, Donnelly N, Hadwin JA, Brown T. Do children with autism perceive second-order
relational
features? The case of the Thatcher illusion. J Child Psychological Psychiatry. 2004 Oct;
45(7):1246
-57. doi : 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00317.x. PMID: 15335345.
pág. 7334
10.
Simion F, Leo I, Turati C, Valenza E, Dalla Barba B. How facial specialization emerges in the
first
months of life. ProgBrain Res. 2007;164:169-85. doi : 10.1016/S0079-6123(07)64009-6.
PMID:
17920431.
11.
Ritz NL, Brocka M, Butler MI, Cowan CSM, Barrera-Bugueño C, Turkington CJR, Draper LA,
Bastiaanssen
TFS, Turpin V, Morales L, Campos D, Gheorghe CE, Ratsika A, Sharma V,
Golubeva
AV, Aburto MR, Shkoporov AN, Moloney GM, Hill C, Clarke G, SlateryDA , Dinan
TG,
Cryan JF. Gut microbiota associated with social anxiety disorder enhances social fear.
ProcNatlAcadSci
US A. 2024 Jan 2;121(1):e2308706120. doi : 10.1073/pnas.2308706120. Epub
2023
Dec 26. PMID: 38147649; PMCID: PMC10769841.
12.
Antonelli-Salgado T, Ramos-Lima LF, Machado CDS, Cassidy RM, Cardoso TA, Kapczinski F,
Passos
IC. Neuroprogression in post-traumatic stress disorder: a systematic review. Trends
Psychiatry
Psychother. 2021 Jul- Sep; 43(3):167-176. doi : 10.47626/2237-6089-2020-0099.
Epub
2021 Apr 16. PMID: 33872477; PMCID: PMC8638708.
13.
Pavlova M, Guerreschi M, Lutzenberger W, Sokolov AN, Krägeloh-Mann I. Cortical response to
social
interaction affected by gender. Neuroimaging. 2010 Apr 15; 50(3):1327-32. doi :
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.096.
Epub 2010 Jan 4 . PMID: 20056153.
14.
Knox J. Response to 'Emotions in action through the looking glass'. J Anal Psychol. 2010 Feb;
55(1):30
-4. doi : 10.1111/j.1468-5922.2009.01822.x. PMID: 20433494.
15.
Rozzi S, Ferrari PF, Bonini L, Rizzolatti G, Fogassi L. Functional organization of the inferior
parietal
lobe convexity in the monkey: electrophysiological characterization of motor, sensory
and
mirror responses and their correlation with cytoarchitectonic areas. Eur J Neurosci. 2008 Oct;
28(8):1569
-88. doi : 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06395.x.Epub 2008 Aug 7. PMID: 18691325.
16.
Bonini L, Rozzi S, Serventi FU, Simone L, Ferrari PF, Fogassi L. The ventral premotor and
inferior
parietal cortices contribute distinctly to the organization of action and the understanding
of
intention. Cerebral Cortex. 2010 Jun; 20(6):1372-85. doi : 10.1093/cercor/bhp200. Epub 2009
Oct
5. PMID: 19805419.
17.
Sinigaglia C, Sparaci L. Emotions in action through the looking glass. J Anal Psychol. 2010
Feb
;55 (1):3-29. doi : 10.1111/j.1468-5922.2009.01821.x. PMID: 20433493.
pág. 7335
18.
Marchman VA, Ashland MD, Loi EC, Adams KA, Fernald A, Feldman HM. Predictors of early
vocabulary
growth in preterm and full-term children: A study of processing speed and medical
complications.
Child Neuropsychol. 2019 Oct; 25(7):943-963. doi :
10.1080/092
97049.2019.1569608. Epub 2019 Feb 4. PMID: 30714476; PMCID: PMC6613975.
19.
Danilov IV, Mihailova S. A New Perspective on Assessing Cognition in Children through
Estimating
Shared Intentionality. J Intell. 2022 Mar 29; 10(2):21. doi :
10.3390/jintelligence10020021.
PMID: 35466234; PMCID: PMC9036231.
20.
Blum K, McLaughlin T, Bowirrat A, Modestino EJ, Baron D, Gomez LL, Ceccanti M, Braverman
ER,
Thanos PK, Cadet JL, Elman I, Badgaiyan RD, Jalali R, Green R, Simpatico TA, Gupta A,
Gold
MS. Reward Deficiency Syndrome (RDS) Is Surprisingly Evolutionary and Found
Everywhere:
Is It "Blowin' in the Wind"? J Pers Med. 2022 Feb 21 ;12 (2):321. doi :
10.3390/jpm12020321.
PMID: 35207809; PMCID: PMC8875142.
21.
Blum K, Atzmon G, Baron D, Badgaiyan RD. Grigorenko EL, De Young CG, Eastman M,
Getchell
M, Haeffel GJ, Klinteberg Ba, Koposov RA, Oreland L, Pakstis AJ, Ponomarev OA,
Ruchkin
VV, Singh JP, Yrigollen CM. Aggressive behavior, related conduct problems, and
v
ariation in genes that affect dopamine turnover. Behaving Aggressive. 2010 May- Jun;
36(3):158
-76. doi : 10.1002/ab.20339. PMID: 20127808. SOJ Psychol. 2016 Oct 17; 3(2):1-5.
doi
: 10.15226/2374-6874/3/2/00129. PMID: 34708151; PMCID: PMC8547313.
22.
Blum K, Bowirrat A, Baron D, Lott L, Ponce JV, Brewer R, Siwicki D, Boyett B, Gondre-Lewis
MC,
Smith DE, Panayotis KT, Badgaiyan S, Hauser M, Fried L, AR 3rd, Downs BW, Badgaiyan
RD.
Biotechnical development of genetic addiction risk score (GARS) and selective evidence for
inclusion
of polymorphic allelic risk in substance use disorder (SUD). J Syst IntegrNeurosci. 2020
Aug
;6 (2):10.15761/JSIN.1000221. doi : 10.15761/JSIN.1000221. Epub 2019 Dec 19. PMID:
33614164;
PMCID: PMC7891477.
23.
Blum, Kenneth et al. “Genetic Addiction Risk Score (GARS): molecular neurogenetic evidence
for
predisposition to Reward Deficiency Syndrome (RDS).” Molecular neurobiology vol. 50.3
(2014):
765-96 . doi:10.1007/s12035-014-8726-5
pág. 7336
24.
Blum K, Chen TJ, Downs BW, Bowirrat A, Waite RL, Braverman ER, Madigan M, Oscar-Berman
M,
DiNubile N, Stice E, Giordano J, Morse S, Gold M. Neurogenetics of dopaminergic receptor
supersensitivity
in activation of brain reward circuitry and relapse: proposing "deprivation-
amplification
relapse therapy" (DART). Postgrad Med. 2009 Nov ;121 (6):176-96. doi :
10.3810/pgm.2009.11.2087.
PMID: 19940429; PMCID: PMC3656125.
25.
Blum K, Kazmi S, Modestino EJ, Downs BW, Bagchi D, Baron D, McLaughlin T, Green R, Jalali
R,
Thanos PK, Elman I, Badgaiyan RD, Bowirrat A, Gold MS. A Novel Precision Approach to
Overcoming
the "Addiction Pandemic" by Incorporating Genetic Addiction Risk Severity
(GARS)
and Dopamine Homeostasis Restoration. J Pers Med 2021 Mar 16 ;11 (3):212. doi :
10.3390/jpm11030212.
PMID: 33809702; PMCID: PMC8002215.
26.
Confavreux B, Vogels TP. A familiar thought: Machines that replace us? Neuron. 2022 Feb
2
;110(3):361-362. doi : 10.1016/j.neuron.2022.01.014. PMID: 35114107.
27.
Thomson G, Flacking R, George K, Feeley N, Haslund-Thomsen H, De Coen K, Schmied V,
Provenzi
L, Rowe J. Parents' experiences of emotional closeness with their infants in the neonatal
unit:
a meta-ethnography. Early Hum Dev. 2020 Oct; 149:105155. doi :
10.1
016/j.earlhumdev.2020.105155. Epub 2020 Aug 14. PMID: 32829240.
28.
Stevens AL, Herrenkohl TI, Mason WA, Smith GL, Klevens J, Merrick MT. Developmental
effects
of childhood household adversity, transitions, and relationship quality on adult outcomes
of
socioeconomic status: Effects of substantiated child maltreatment. Child Abuse Negl. 2018
May;
79:42-50. doi :10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.01.031. Epub 2018 Feb 6. PMID:29407855;
PMCID:PMC6134210.

29.
Khoury JE, Bosquet Enlow M, Plamondon A, Lyons-Ruth K. The association between adversity
and
hair cortisol levels in humans: A meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2019
May
;103:104 -117. doi : 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.01.009. Epub 2019 Jan 11. PMID: 30682626;
PMCID:
PMC6450779.
30.
Oh DL, Jerman P, Silvério Marques S, Koita K, PurewalBoparai SK, Burke Harris N, Bucci M.
Systematic
review of pediatric health outcomes associated with childhood adversities. BMC
pág. 7337
Pediatrics.
2018 Feb 23; 18(1):83. doi : 10.1186/s12887-018-1037-7. PMID: 29475430; PMCID:
PMC5824569.

31.
Yee AZ, Lwin MO, Ho SS. The influence of parenting practices on children's promotional and
preventive
food consumption behaviors: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J BehavNutr
Phys
Act. 2017 Apr 11 ;14 (1):47. doi : 10.1186/s12966-017-0501-3. PMID: 28399881; PMCID:
PMC5387370.

32.
Jaswetz L, de Voogd LD, Becker ES, Roelofs K. No evidence for disruption of reconsolidation of
conditioned
threat memories with a cognitively demanding intervention. Science Rep. 2022 Apr
22;
12(1):6663. doi : 10.1038/s41598-022-10184-1. PMID: 35459769; PMCID: PMC9033821.
33.
Blum K, Baron D, McLaughlin T, Gold MS. Molecular neurological correlates of
endorphinergic/dopaminergic
mechanisms in reward circuitry linked to endorphinergic
deficiency
syndrome (EDS). J Neurol Sci. 2020 Apr 15; 411:116733. doi :
10.1016/j.jns.2020.1167
33. Epub 2020 Feb 14. PMID: 32088516.
34.
Palma-Gudiel H, Córdova-Palomera A, Eixarch E, Deuschle M, Fañanás L. Maternal
psychosocial
stress during pregnancy alters the epigenetic signature of the glucocorticoid receptor
gene
promoter in their offspring: a meta-analysis. Epigenetics. 2015; 10(10):893-902. doi :
10.1080/15592294.2015.1088630.
PMID: 26327302; PMCID: PMC4844196.
35.
Schrott R, Song A, Ladd-Acosta C. Epigenetics as a biomarker for early-life environmental
exposure.
Curr Environ Health Rep. 2022 Dec ;9 (4):604-624. doi : 10.1007/s40572-022-00373-
5.
Epub 2022 Jul 30. PMID: 35907133.
36.
Golds L, Gillespie-Smith K, Nimbley E, MacBeth A. What factors influence dyadic synchrony?
A
systematic review of the literature on predictors of mother-infant dyadic processes of shared
behavior
and affect. Infant Ment Health J. 2022 Sep ;43 (5):808-830. doi : 10.1002/imhj.22011.
Epub
2022 Aug 1. PMID: 35913364; PMCID: PMC9540815.
37.
Bell MA. Mother-child behavioral and physiological synchrony. Adv Child Dev Behav.
2020
;58:163 -188. doi : 10.1016/bs.acdb.2020.01.006. Epub 2020 Jan 27. PMID: 32169195.
pág. 7338
38.
Lieberwirth C, Wang Z. The neurobiology of pair bond formation, bond disruption, and social
buffering.
CurrOpinNeurobiol. 2016 Oct ;40:8 -13. doi : 10.1016/j.conb.2016.05.006. Epub 2016
Jun
9. PMID: 27290660; PMCID: PMC5072360.
39.
Lee TH, Miernicki ME, Telzer EH. Families that fire together smile together: Resting state
connectome
similarity and daily emotional synchrony in parent-child dyads. Neuroimaging. 2017
May
15 ;152:31 -37. doi : 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.02.078. Epub 2017 Feb 28. PMID:
28254510;
PMCID: PMC5555750.
40.
Endevelt-Shapira Y, Djalovski A, Dumas G, Feldman R. Maternal chemosignals enhance infant-
adult
brain-to-brain synchrony. Sci Adv. 2021 Dec 10 ;7 (50):eabg6867. doi :
10.1126/sciadv.abg6867.
Epub 2021 Dec 10. PMID: 34890230; PMCID: PMC8664266.
41.
Carollo A, Lim M, Aryadoust V, Esposito G. Interpersonal Synchrony in the Context of Caregiver-
Child
Interactions: A Document Co-citation Analysis. Front Psychol. 2021 Jul 28 ;12:701824 .
doi
: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.701824. PMID: 34393940; PMCID: PMC8355520.
42.
Leslie CE, Walsh CS, Sullivan TN. Implications of intergenerational trauma: Associations
between
caregiver ACEs and child internalizing symptoms in an urban African American sample.
Psychol
Trauma. 2023 Jul; 15(5):877-887. doi : 10.1037/tra0001334. Epub 2022 Jul 28. PMID:
35901424;
PMCID: PMC10191152.
43.
Lee TH, Miernicki ME, Telzer EH. Families that fire together smile together: Resting state
connectome
similarity and daily emotional synchrony in parent-child dyads. Neuroimaging. 2017
May
15 ;152:31 -37. doi : 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.02.078. Epub 2017 Feb 28. PMID:
28254510;
PMCID: PMC5555750.
44.
Saltzman WR, Pynoos RS, Lester P, Layne CM, Beardslee WR. Enhancing family resilience
through
co-construction of family narrative. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2013 Sep ;16 (3):294-
310.
doi : 10.1007/s10567-013-014-2. PMID: 23797387.
45.
Savarimuthu A, Ponniah RJ. Receiving, Retaining, and Retrieving: Psychological and
Neurobiological
Perspectives on Memory Retrieval. IntegrPsycholBehaportSci . 2024 Mar;
58(1):303
-318. doi : 10.1007/s12124-023-09752-5. Epub 2023 Feb 4. PMID: 36738400.
pág. 7339
46.
Gao W, Cui Z, Yu Y, Mao J, Xu J, Ji L, Kan X, Shen X, Li X, Zhu S, Hong Y. Application of a
Brain
-Computer Interface System with Visual and Motor Feedback in Functional Rehabilitation
of
Limbs and Brain after Stroke: A Case Report. Brain Science. 2022 Aug 16; 12(8):1083. doi :
10.3390/brainsci12081083.
PMID: 36009146; PMCID: PMC9405856.
47.
Lyons-Ruth K, Spielman E. DISORGANIZED INFANT ATTACHMENT STRATEGIES AND
HELPLESS
AND FEARFUL PARENTING PROFILES : INTEGRATING ATTACHMENT
RESEARCH
WITH CLINICAL INTERVENTION. Child Health J. 2004; 25(4):318-335. doi :
10.1002/imhj.20008.
PMID: 17464363; PMCID: PMC1857278.
48.
Krause AL, Colic L, Borchardt V, Li M, Strauss B, Buchheim A, Wildgruber D, Fonagy P, Nolte
T,
Walter M. Functional connectivity changes following interpersonal reactivity. Hum
BrainMapp
. 2018 Feb; 39(2):866-879. doi : 10.1002/hbm.23888. Epub 2017 Nov 21 PMID:
29164726;
PMCID: PMC6866275.
49.
Gunnar MR, Hostinar CE, Sanchez MM, Tottenham N, Sullivan RM. Parental buffering of fear
and
stress neurobiology: Reviewing parallels across rodent, monkey, and human models. Soc
Neurosci.
2015; 10(5):474-8. doi : 10.1080/17470919.2015.1070198. Epub 2015 Aug 25. PMID:
26234160;
PMCID: PMC5198892.
50.
Chaudhary S, Zhornitsky S, Chao HH, van Dyck CH, Li CR. Emotion Processing Dysfunction in
Alzheimer's
Disease: An Overview of Behavioral Findings, Systems Neural Correlates, and
Underlying
Neural Biology. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen. 2022 Jan- Dec;
37:
15333175221082834. doi : 10.1177/15333175221082834. PMID: 35357236; PMCID:
PMC9212074.

51.
Grogans SE, Bliss-Moreau E, Buss KA, Clark LA, Fox AS, Keltner D, Cowen AS, Kim JJ, Kragel
PA,
MacLeod C, Mobbs D, Naragon-Gainey K, Fullana MA, Shackman AJ. The nature and
neurobiology
of fear and anxiety: State of the science and opportunities for accelerating
discovery
. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2023 Aug; 151:105237. doi :
10.1016/j.neubiorev.2023.105237.
Epub 2023 May 18. PMID: 37209932; PMCID:
PMC10330657.
pág. 7340
52.
Blum K, Chen TJ, Chen AL, Madigan M, Downs BW, Waite RL, Braverman ER, Kerner M,
Bowirrat
A, Giordano J, Henshaw H, Gold MS. Do dopaminergic gene polymorphisms affect
mesolimbic
reward activation of music listening response? Therapeutic impact on Reward
De
ficiency Syndrome (RDS). Med Hypotheses. 2010 Mar; 74(3):513-20. doi :
10.1016/j.mehy.2009.10.008.
Epub 2009 Nov 14. PMID: 19914781.
53.
Blum K, Braverman ER, Holder JM, Lubar JF, Monastra VJ, Miller D, Lubar JO, Chen TJ,
Comings
DE. Reward deficiency syndrome: a biogenetic model for the diagnosis and treatment
of
impulsive, addictive, and compulsive behaviors. J Psychoactive Drugs. 2000 Nov; 32 Suppl:i-
iv,
1-112. doi : 10.1080/02791072.2000.10736099. PMID: 11280926.
54.
Blum K, Sheridan PJ, Wood RC, Braverman ER, Chen TJ, Comings DE. Dopamine D2 receptor
gene
variants: association and linkage studies in impulsive-addictive-compulsive behavior.
Pharmacogenetics.
1995 Jun; 5(3):121-41. doi : 10.1097/00008571-199506000-00001. PMID:
7550364.

55.
Blum K, Chen AL, Oscar-Berman M, Chen TJ, Lubar J, White N, Lubar J, Bowirrat A, Braverman
E,
Schoolfield J, Waite RL, Downs BW, Madigan M, Comings DE, Davis C, Kerner MM, Knopf
J,
Palomo T, Giordano JJ, Morse SA, Fornari F, Barh D, Femino J, Bailey JA. Generational
association
studies of dopaminergic genes in reward deficiency syndrome (RDS) subjects:
selecting
appropriate phenotypes for reward dependence behaviors. Int J Environ Res Public
Health.
2011 Dec; 8(12):4425-59. doi : 10.3390/ijerph8124425. Epub 2011 Nov 29. PMID:
22408582;
PMCID: PMC3290972.
56.
Blum K, Gold MS, Cadet JL, Gondre-Lewis MC, McLaughlin T, Braverman ER, Elman I, Paul
Carney
B, Cortese R, Abijo T, Bagchi D, Giordano J, Dennen CA, Baron D, Thanos PK, Soni D ,
Makale
MT, Makale M, Murphy KT, Jafari N, Sunder K, Zeine F, Ceccanti M, Bowirrat A,
Badgaiyan
RD. Invited Expert Opinion- Bioinformatics and Limitation Directives to Help Adopt
Genetic
Addiction Risk Screening and Identify Preaddictive Reward Dysregulation: Required
Analytic
Evidence to Induce Dopamine Homeostatsis. Med Res Arch. 2023 Sep 14;
11(8):10.18103/mra.v11i8.4211.
doi : 10.18103/mra.v11i8.4211. PMID: 37885438; PMCID:
PMC10601302.
pág. 7341
57.
Nieuwenhuys R. The myeloarchitectonic studies on the human cerebral cortex of the Vogt-Vogt
school
and their significance for the interpretation of functional neuroimaging data. Brain
Structure
Function. 2013 Mar; 218(2):303-52. doi : 10.1007/s00429-012-0460-z. Epub 2012 Oct
18.
PMID: 23076375.
58.
Pavlova M. Perception and understanding of intentions and actions: does gender matter?
NeurosciLett
. 2009 Jan 9 ;449(2):133-6. doi : 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.10.102. Epub 2008 Nov 5.
PMID:
19007857.
59.
Pavlova M, Sokolov AN, Birbaumer N, Krägeloh-Mann I. Perception and understanding of
others'
actions and brain connectivity. J CognNeurosci . 2008 Mar; 20(3):494-504. doi :
10.1162/jocn.2008.20034.
PMID: 18004956.
60.
Pavlova MA, Krägeloh-Mann I. Limitations in preterm brain development: impact of
periventricular
white matter lesions on brain connectivity and cognition. Brain. 2013 Apr; 136(Pt
4):998
-1011. doi : 10.1093/brain/aws334. PMID: 23550112.
61.
Pavlova M, Sokolov A, Krägeloh-Mann I. Visual navigation in adolescents with early
periventricular
lesions: knowing where but not getting there. Cerebral Cortex. 2007 Feb;
17(2):363
-9. doi : 10.1093/cercor/bhj153. Epub 2006 Mar 8. PMID: 16525128.
62.
Marchman VA, Ashland MD, Loi EC, Munevar M, Shannon KA, Fernald A, Feldman HM.
Associations
between early language processing efficiency and cognitive and language outcomes
in
term and preterm children: Similarities and differences. NeuropsycholInfantil . 2023 Aug;
29(6):886
-905. doi : 10.1080/09297049.2022.2138304 . Epub 2022 Nov 2. PMID: 36324057;
PMCID:
PMC10151433.
63.
Pavlova M, Sokolov AN, Krägeloh-Mann I. Arithmetic and brain connectivity: mental calculation
in
adolescents with periventricular lesions. Neuropsychology. 2009 Jan; 47(2):439-45. doi :
10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.09.014.
Epub 2008 Sep 25. PMID: 18929585.
64.
Herpertz SC, Bertsch K, Jeung H. Neurobiology of Criterion A: self and interpersonal personality
functioning.
Curr Opin Psychol. 2018 Jun; 21:23-27. doi : 10.016/j.copsyc.2017.08.032. Epub
2017
Sep 9 . PMID: 28946053.
pág. 7342
65.
Samuel G. The contemporary mindfulness movement and the question of not-self1. Transculta
Psychiatry.
2015 Aug; 52(4):485-500. doi : 10.1177/1363461514562061. Epub 2014 Dec 5.
PMID:
25480489.
66.
Panaïoti A. Mindfulness and personal identity in the Western cultural context: A call for greater
cosmopolitanism.
Transculta Psychiatry. 2015 Aug; 52(4):501-23. doi :
10.1177/1363461515573106.
Epub 2015 Mar 12. PMID: 25765440.
67.
Doehrmann O, Ghosh SS, Polli FE, Reynolds GO, Horn F, Keshavan A, Triantafyllou C, Saygin
ZM,
Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Hofmann SG, Pollack M, Gabrieli JD. Predicting treatment response
in
social anxiety disorder from functional magnetic resonance imaging. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013
Jan;
70(1):87-97. doi : 10.1001/2013.jamapsychiatry.5. PMID: 22945462; PMCID:
PMC3844518.

68.
Bonini L, Rozzi S, Serventi FU, Simone L, Ferrari PF, Fogassi L. The ventral premotor and
inferior
parietal cortices contribute distinctly to the organization of action and the understanding
of
intention. Cerebral Cortex. 2010 Jun; 20(6): 1372-85. doi : 10.1093/cercor/bhp200. Epub 2009
Oct
5. PMID: 19805419.
69.
Uylings HB, Rajkowska G, Sanz-Arigita E, Amunts K, Zilles K. Consequences of large
interindividual
variability for human brain atlases: convergent macroscopic imaging and
microscopic
neuroanatomy. AnatEmbryol (Berl). 2005 Dec ;210 (5-6):423-31. doi :
10.10
07/s00429-005-0042-4. PMID: 16180019.
70.
Bonnel A, Mottron L, Peretz I, Trudel M, Gallun E, Bonnel AM. Enhanced tone sensitivity in
individuals
with autism: a signal detection analysis. J CognNeurosci . 2003 Feb 15; 15(2):226-
35.
doi :10.1162/089892903321208169. PMID:12676060.
71.
Márquez C, Poirier GL, Cordero MI, Larsen MH, Groner A, Marquis J, Magistretti PJ, Trono D,
Sandi
C. Peripubertal stress leads to abnormal aggression, altered amygdala and orbitofrontal
reactivity,
and increased prefrontal MAOA gene expression. Psychiatry Transl. 2013 Jan 15;
3(1):e216.
doi : 10.1038/tp.2012.144. PMID: 23321813; PMCID: PMC3566724.
72.
Thomson G, Flacking R, George K, Feeley N, Haslund-Thomsen H, De Coen K, Schmied V,
Provenzi
L, Rowe J. Parents' experiences of emotional closeness with their infants in the neonatal
pág. 7343
unit:
a meta-ethnography. Early Hum Dev. 2020 Oct; 149:105155. doi :
10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2020.105155.
Epub 2020 Aug 14. PMID: 32829240.
73.
Stevens AL, Herrenkohl TI, Mason WA, Smith GL, Klevens J, Merrick MT. Developmental
effects
of childhood household adversity, transitions, and relationship quality on adult outcomes
of
socioeconomic status: Effects of substantiated child maltreatment. Child Abuse Negl. 2018
May;
79:42-50. doi :10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.01.031. Epub 2018 Feb 6. PMID:29407855;
PMCID:PMC6134210.

74.
Khoury JE, Bosquet Enlow M, Plamondon A, Lyons-Ruth K. The association between adversity
and
hair cortisol levels in humans: A meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2019
May
;103:104 -117. doi : 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.01.009. Epub 2019 Jan 11. PMID: 30682626;
PMCID:
PMC6450779.
75.
Oh DL, Jerman P, Silvério Marques S, Koita K, PurewalBoparai SK, Burke Harris N, Bucci M.
Systematic
review of pediatric health outcomes associated with childhood adversities. BMC
Pediatrics.
2018 Feb 23; 18(1):83. doi : 10.1186/s12887-018-1037-7. PMID: 29475430; PMCID:
PMC5824569.

76.
Yee AZ, Lwin MO, Ho SS. The influence of parenting practices on children's promotional and
preventive
food consumption behaviors: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J BehavNutr
Phys
Act. 2017 Apr 11 ;14 (1):47. doi : 10.1186/s12966-017-0501-3. PMID: 28399881; PMCID:
PMC5387370.

77.
Jaswetz L, de Voogd LD, Becker ES, Roelofs K. No evidence for disruption of reconsolidation of
conditioned
threat memories with a cognitively demanding intervention. Science Rep. 2022 Apr
22;
12(1):6663. doi : 10.1038/s41598-022-10184-1. PMID: 35459769; PMCID: PMC9033821.
78.
Cirillo A, Diniz E, Gadelha A, Asevedo E, Axelrud LK, Miguel EC, Rohde LA, Bressan RA, Pan
P,
Mari JJ. Population neuroscience: challenges and opportunities for psychiatric research in low-
and
middle-income countries. Psychiatry Braz J. 2020 Aug; 42(4):442-448. doi : 10.1590/1516-
4446
-2019-0761. Epub 2020 Apr 3. PMID: 32267341; PMCID: PMC7430393.
pág. 7344
79.
Anker JJ, Kushner MG. Alcohol Use Disorder and Anxiety: Connecting Psychiatric,
Psychological,
and Neurobiological Perspectives. Alcohol Res. 2019 Dec 30 ;
40(1):arcr.v40.1.03.
doi : 10.35946/arcr.v40.1.03. PMID: 31886106; PMCID: PMC6927748.
80.
2020 Mar 18 ; 10(3):173. doi : 10.3390/brainsci10030173. PMID: 32197333; PMCID:
PMC7139956.

81.
Ceban F, Ling S, Lui LMW, Lee Y, Gill H, Teopiz KM, Rodrigues NB, Subramaniapillai M, Di
Vincenzo
JD, Cao B, Lin K, Mansur RB, Ho RC, Rosenblat JD, Miskowiak KW, Vinberg M,
Maletic
V, McIntyre RS. Fatigue and cognitive impairment in post-COVID-19 syndrome: A
systematic
review and meta-analysis. Brain Behavior Immune. 2022 Mar; 101:93-135. doi :
10.1016/j.bbi.2021.12.020.
Epub 2021 Dec 29. PMID: 34973396; PMCID: PMC8715665.
82.
Turkish Association for Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 2020 ). r J Psychol Med. 2020
Sep;37(3):222
-225. doi : 10.1017/ipm.2020.57. Epub 2020 May 21. PMID: 32434621; PMCID:
PMC7276500.

83.
Geugies H, Mocking RJT, Figueroa CA, Groot PFC, Marsman JC, Servaas MN, Steele JD,
Schene
AH, Ruhé HG. Impaired reward-related learning cues in non-medicated remitted patients
with
recurrent depression. Brain. 2019 Aug 1; 142(8):2510-2522. doi : 10.1093/brain/awz167.
PMID:
31280309; PMCID: PMC6734943.
84.
Hornor G. Attachment disorders. J Pediatr Health Care. 2019 Sep-Oct ;33 (5):612-622. doi :
10.1016/j.pedhc.2019.04.017.
Erratum in: J Pediatr Health Care. 2020 Jan - Feb ;34 (1):82.
PMID:
31447013.
85.
Bellis MA, Hughes K, Ford K, Ramos Rodriguez G, Sethi D, Passmore J. Life course health
consequences
and associated annual costs of adverse childhood experiences across Europe and
North
America: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Public Health. 2019 Oct ;4
(10):e517
-e528. doi : 10.1016/S2468-2667(19)30145-8. Epub 2019 Sep 3. PMID: 31492648;
PMCID:
PMC7098477.
86.
Tough H, Siegrist J, Fekete C. Social relationships, mental health and wellbeing in physical
disability:
a systematic review. BMC Public Health. 2017 May 8 ;17 (1):414. doi :
pág. 7345
10.1186/s12889
-017-4308-6. Erratum in: BMC Public Health. 2017 Jun 16 ;17 (1)580. PMID:
28482878;
PMCID: PMC5422915.
87.
Yee AZ, Lwin MO, Ho SS. The influence of parenting practices on children's promotional and
preventive
food consumption behaviors: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J BehavNutr
Phys
Act. 2017 Apr 11 ;14 (1):47. doi : 10.1186/s12966-017-0501-3. PMID: 28399881; PMCID:
PMC5387370.

88.
Mah BL. Oxytocin, Postnatal Depression, and Parenting: A Systematic Review. Harv Rev
Psychiatry.
2016 Jan-Feb ;24 (1):1-13. doi : 10.1097/HRP.00000000000000093. PMID:
26735320
.
89.
Ciuffo ICG, LandoniM,.ParentInfant Skin-to-Skin Contact and Stress Regulation: A Systematic
Review
of the Literature
90.
Bellis MA, Hughes K, Ford K, Ramos Rodriguez G, Sethi D, Passmore J. Life course health
consequences
and associated annual costs of adverse childhood experiences across Europe and
North
America: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Public Health. 2019 Oct ;4
(10):e517
-e528. doi : 10.1016/S2468-2667(19)30145-8. Epub 2019 Sep 3. PMID: 31492648;
PMCID:
PMC7098477.
91.
Warner TD, Leban L, Pester DA, Walker JT. Contextualizing Adverse Childhood Experiences:
The
Intersections of Individual and Community Adversity. J Youth Adolescence. 2023 Mar;
52(3):570
-584. doi : 10.1007/s10964-022-01713-2. Epub 2022 Nov 29. PMID: 36445650.
92.
Leisman G, Machado C, Melillo R, Mualem R. Intentionality and "free-will" from a
neurodevelopmental
perspective. Front Integration Neurosci. 2012 Jun 27; 6:36. doi :
10.3389/fnint.2012.00036.
PMID: 22754510; PMCID: PMC3385506.
93.
Hong MY, Lee SW, Kim EY. Relationship of Momentary Volition to Occupational Experience
and
Life Perspective in Undergraduate Students. Healthcare (Basel). 2023 Sep 5 ;11(18):2471.
doi
: 10.3390/healthcare11182471. PMID: 37761668; PMCID: PMC10530513.
94.
Ong HS, Fernandez PA, Lim HK. Family engagement as part of managing patients with mental
illness
in primary care. Singapore Med J. 2021 May ;62 (5):213-219. doi :
10.11622/smedj.2021057.
PMID: 34409463; PMCID: PMC8801858.
pág. 7346
95.
Schladitz K, Weitzel EC, Löbner M, Soltmann B, Jessen F, Schmitt J, Pfennig A, Riedel-Heller
SG,
Gühne U. Demands on Health Information and Clinical Practice Guidelines for Patients from
the
Perspective of Adults with Mental Illness and Family Members : A Qualitative Study with In-
Depth
Interviews. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2022 Nov 1 ;19 (21):14262. doi :
10.3390/ijerph192114262.
PMID: 36361142; PMCID: PMC9659184.
96.
Blair C, Raver CC. Child development in the context of adversity: experiential channeling of
brain
and behavior. Am Psychol. 2012 May-Jun ;67 (4):309-18. doi : 10.1037/a0027493. Epub
2012
Mar 5. PMID: 22390355 ;PMCID : PMC5264526.
97.
Bates RA, Militello L, Barker E, Villasanti HG, Schmeer K. Early childhood stress responses to
psychosocial
stressors: The state of the science. Dev Psychobiol. 2022 Nov ;64 (7):e22320. doi :
10.1002/dev.22320.
PMID: 36282746; PMCID: PMC9543576.
98.
Garvin MM, Bolton JL. Sex-specific behavioral outcomes of early-life adversity and emerging
microglia
-dependent mechanisms. Front BehavNeurosci . 2022 Oct 4 ;16:1013865. doi :
10.3389/fnbeh.2022.1013865.
PMID: 36268470; PMCID: PMC9577368.
99.
Aguilera M, Arias B, Wichers M, Barrantes-Vidal N, Moya J, Villa H, van Os J, Ibáñez MI,
Ruipérez
MA, Ortet G, Fañanás L. Early adversity and 5-HTT/BDNF genes: new evidence for
gene
-environment interactions in depressive symptoms in a general population. Psychol Med.
2009
Sep; 39(9):1425-32. doi : 10.1017/S0033291709005248. Epub 2009 Feb 12. PMID:
19215635.

100.
Murthy BKB, Somatakis S, Ulivi AF, Klimmt H, Castello-Waldow TP, Haynes N, Huettl RE,
Chen
A, Attardo A. Arc-driven mGRASP highlights CA1 to CA3 synaptic engrams. Front
Behavior
Neurosci. 2023 Jan 30; 16:1072571. doi : 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.1072571. PMID:
367
93796; PMCID: PMC9924068.
101.
Gastaldi C, Schwalger T, De Falco E, Quiroga RQ, Gerstner W. When shared concept cells
support
associations: Theory of overlapping memory engrams. PLoS Comput Biol. 2021 Dec
30;17(12):e1009691.
doi : 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1009691. PMID: 34968383; PMCID:
PMC
8754331.
pág. 7347
102.
Cui Y, Liu Y, Yang C, Cui C, Jing D, Zhang X, Chen Y, Li B, Liang Z, Chen K, Zhang Z, Wu L.
Brain
structural and functional anomalies associated with simultanagnosia in patients with
posterior
cortical atrophy . Brain Imaging Behavior. 2022 Jun; 16(3):1148-1162. doi :
10.1007/s11682
-021-00568-8. Epub 2021 Nov 17. PMID: 34787788; PMCID: PMC9107404.
103.
Darby, R. R., Edersheim, J. , & Price, B. H. (2016). What patients with behavioral variant
frontotemporal
dementia can teach us about moral responsibility. AJOB Neuroscience , 7 (4),
193
201.
104.
Skipper JI. A voice without a mouth no more: The neurobiology of language and consciousness.
NeurosciBiobehav
Rev. 2022 Sep ;140:104772 . doi : 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104772 . Epub
2022
Jul 11. PMID: 35835286.
105.
Karim AKMR, Proulx MJ, de Sousa AA, Likova LT. Do we enjoy what we sense and perceive?
A
dissociation between aesthetic appreciation and basic perception of environmental objects or
events.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2022 Oct; 22(5):904-951. doi : 10.3758/s13415-022-01004-
0.
Epub 2022 May 19. PMID: 35589909; PMCID: PMC10159614.
106.
Furlanetto MLJr.(2023). Dialectical Medicina: Syndrome Z and Use of Pathological Substances.
J.
Integrated Health. 2(1),1-20 2 ISSN:2583-5386
107.
Furlanetto M.L.Jr.(2023). Clinical Neurogenetics of syndrome Z and Scientific Evidence Based
Clinical
Evaluation. J Health Integrated. 2(1), 21-25.DOI: http//:
doi.org/10.51219/JIH/Furlanetto
-M-L-Jr/5
108.
Furlanetto M.L.Jr(2023). Stakehold ZXSY Brazil,Precision Mediicine in Pediatric Clinic:
Reward
Deficiency Syndrome (RDS)is surprisingly Evolving and Found Everywhere: Is It
´´lowing
in the Wind? Yes, but how Syndrome Z.J. Integrated Health,2(2),26-
38.DOI:doi.org/10.51219/JIH/Furlanetto
-M-L-Jr-6
109.
Ivan T., et al (2023).Precision Medicine in Pediatrics: Infantile Z Syndrome, Familial Sync
Neurological
Deficit, ANAAS Syndrome, Zoé Syndrome, RDS Spectrum Disorder. J Medical
Case
Repo, 5(2):1-15.
110.
M. L. Furlanetto Junior (2023). Stakehold ZXSY: Disorder Deficit Familial Asynchrony and
Syndrome
Z. J Medical Case Repo, 5(1):1-24. DOI: https://doi.org/10.47485/2767-5416.1032
pág. 7348
111.
Detrigiachi E, Sukorski JP, de Oliveira MTM, Lima LA,Furlanetto Junior ML
(
2023):Medicine,Law and Collective Mental Health:New Therapeutic for pré- parental and Pré-
addiction.J.Integrated
Health20223;2(4)91-99.DOI:doi.org/10.51219/Mario-Luiz-16
112.
Luiz Furlanetto Junior et al.Diagnostic Methodologic and Prevention on Internal Medicine and
common
Neurobehavior:Target of prevention of cardiovascular events and Metabolic syndrome.
Cardiol
Arch 2023; 1(1):5-11.